Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Effects of Visual Working Memory on Individual Differences in Echolocation Performance in Sighted Participants:

Effects of Visual Working Memory on Individual Differences in Echolocation Performance in Sighted... Echolocation performance differs widely among individuals. This study examined a possible factor that may explain this variation, namely, visual working memory, which is a subcomponent of spatial working memory. Sighted participants performed an object-detection task consisting of initial testing on 2 separate days (up to 8 days apart) with follow-up testing on a third day (up to 1 month after the second day of testing) while manipulating the target distance from 20 to 50 cm. Participants performed two types of visual spatial working memory tasks, one of which required them to memorize color–location combinations and the other, an imaginary pathway. The participants’ performance on the object-detection task generally improved in the first 2 days, but there were substantial individual differences in detection ability. A positive correlation was observed between performance on these tasks and visual working memory capacity, except on the second day, after detection ability had improved. These findings suggest that factors contrib- uting to echolocation skill are related to nonauditory factors in a sighted group. Keywords echolocation, perception, visual spatial working memory Date received: 11 March 2019; accepted: 1 August 2019 Introduction Echolocation is a method to localize objects and acquire object features (e.g., distance, size, shape, and surface of material) based on the reflection of sound. Echolocation ability has been Corresponding author: Tomoki Maezawa, Hokkaido University, N10W7, Kita, Sapporo 060-0810, Japan. Email: porpoise@let.hokudai.ac.jp Creative Commons CC BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage). 2 i-Perception 10(4) extensively acknowledged in nonhuman species, such as odontocetes (e.g., Au, Branstetter, Benoit-Bird, & Kastelein, 2009) and some bats (e.g., Barclay & Brigham, 1994; Neuweiler, 1990). Similarly, humans with normal hearing can use echolocation (Rice & Feinstein, 1965; Schenkman & Nilsson, 2010). Particularly, the sighted population can echolocate (Teng & Whitney, 2011) using several types of echo-acoustic sounds, such as oral clicks and artificial click noises via loud speakers (Thaler & Castillo-Serrano, 2016; Tonelli, Brayda, & Gori, 2016; Tonelli, Campus, & Brayda, 2018), as well as auditory substitution to enhance spatial awareness for successful navigation (Kolarik, Scarfe, Moore, & Pardhan, 2016). Echolocation skill can be acquired with suitable training, although most sighted groups are less sensitive to the cues available for echolocation (Kolarik, Cirstea, Pardhan, & Moore, 2014). Rapid improvements in echolocation performance occur in sighted populations fol- lowing only a few training sessions (Teng & Whitney, 2011). These findings indicate the need for repetitive training to acquire echolocation skill. However, large individual differences in echolocation performance and the degree of improvement during echolocation training may complicate the establishment of systematic echolocation training protocols (e.g., Ekkel, van Lier, & Steenbergen, 2017), and some individuals are unable to acquire echolocation skills (e.g., Worchel, Mauney, & Andrew, 1950). Thus, a better understanding of the factors contributing to these individual differences is necessary to aid in the training of echolocation skills. A plausible explanation for the individual differences is that echolocation performance is affected by variation in auditory sensitivity of the hearing system because performance is based on detecting and discriminating auditory echoic cues. A set of studies has supported this relationship (e.g., Carlson-Smith & Wiener, 1996; Schenkman & Nilsson, 2010). Specifically, experiences with echoic information, auditory perceptual learning, and task feedback are associated with echolocation performance (e.g., Teng & Whitney, 2011; Teng, Puri, & Whitney, 2012). However, the ability to discriminate echoic cues is not the only factor that explains the individual variability (Rice, 1967) because sighted populations, in particular, do not rely on echoic cues relative to blind populations. Thus, other factors are involved. In this study, we argue that working memory capacity plays a role in echolocation per- formance. Working memory is the temporary storage and workspace to manipulate infor- mation for ongoing tasks (e.g., Baddeley, 2012; Ricker, AuBuchon, & Cowan, 2010), and functions in conjunction with a subsidiary slave system of the attentional control system, known as the central executive (Baddeley, 2000). The relevance of working memory in audi- tory cognitive processing has been acknowledged (e.g., Moossavi, Mehrkian, Lotfi, Faghihzadeh, & Sajedi, 2014). In fact, the degree of improvement in echolocation perfor- mance positively correlates with a test score for the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT; Gronwall, 1977), reflecting working memory and divided attention (Ekkel et al., 2017), in conjunction with auditory verbal aspects. The PASAT has been used as a measure of impairments in the central executive of working memory (e.g., Serino et al., 2007); thus, the results of Ekkel et al. (2017) support the notion that working memory capacity modulates echolocation performance in size discrimination. However, it should be noted that the indi- vidual differences in the ability to use echolocation was beyond the scope of Ekkel et al.’s study. Rather, their focus was on the differences in improvements in echolocation perfor- mance due to training. Moreover, even though the central executive component would con- tribute to the learning of size discrimination (Ekkel et al., 2017), the processing of echolocation relates to spatial perception; thus, the process should involve the spatial com- ponent of working memory. Thus, further investigation is required to examine whether echolocation performance is linked to the capacity of spatial working memory, rather than central executive functions. Maezawa and Kawahara 3 Visual working memory (Luck & Vogel, 2013; Mance & Vogel, 2013) is a subsidiary system of working memory, contributing to maintenance of visual information to serve the needs of ongoing tasks. The concept of the spatial subcomponent of working memory has played a central role in explaining individual differences in cognitive performance in the spatial domain (Kyllonen & Christal, 1990). Given that high visual working memory capac- ity is predictive of higher performance on visual tasks, such as visual search, localization and detection of changes (e.g., Luria & Vogel, 2011; Unsworth, Fukuda, Awh, & Vogel, 2014), visual working memory should predict individual differences in cognitive performance involving spatial processes. Importantly, visual working memory capacity predicts individ- uals’ filtering efficiency, so that in individuals with high visual working memory capacity, irrelevant distractor-related information is efficiently excluded from storage in memory, thus preserving the capacity for purposeful use (Vogel, McCullough, & Machizawa, 2005). Although the concept of visual working memory has been traditionally related to active maintenance of visual representations, recent studies on working memory have extended the concept to spatial processing of images in auditory modality tasks (Loomis, Klatzky, McHugh, & Giudice, 2012). We hypothesized that visual processing and experience would contribute to the representation of mental images and aid echolocation in sighted subjects. In fact, much effort has been expended on characterizing the relationship between echolo- cation and visual cognitive processing (Tao et al., 2015; Thaler & Foresteire, 2017; Thaler, Wilson, & Gee, 2014). Thus, we expected that sighted individuals with superior visual work- ing memory capacity would perform better in auditory spatial tasks, such as target search and change detection tasks. In other words, individuals with high working memory capacity should be able to detect a target object from distractor noise due to high-filtering efficiency. Thus, the aim of this study was to examine the association between echolocation perfor- mance in sighted individuals and how individual factors of visual working memory capacity contribute to performance. Specifically, we examined whether visual working memory capac- ity correlates with object detection performance in echolocation tasks. We also investigated whether improved detection performance is retained over a month, by asking participants to perform the same echolocation tasks 3 times over that time periods. Maintaining improved echolocation ability after training presents a challenge for echolocation learning. In one study (Zahorik, Bangayan, Sundareswaran, Wang, & Tam, 2006) that did not use an echo- location task, improved ability in sound localization persisted for at least 4 months. Similar retention of echolocation improvement was expected in this study. We designed an object-detection task based on Schenkman and Nilsson (2010), whose participants determined the presence or absence of a target in front of them using echolo- cation, while the distance to the target was manipulated. The experiment lasted 120 trials (approximately 50 minutes) per day across 2 days and the amount of training during the task was greatly reduced from that used by Schenkman and Nilsson (2010), who used 56 trials per session across 36 sessions (2.5–3 hours) with feedback on every trial. We also omitted the feedback. Specifically, our experiment consisted of initial testing on 2 separate days (up to 8 days apart) with a follow-up testing on a third day (up to 1 month apart). Two sighted participants in our pilot study showed a chance level in detecting a target placed more than 50 cm away; thus, we chose a target distance of 20 to 50 cm. This range was shorter than the previous study (Schenkman & Nilsson, 2010) that covered from 50 to 500 cm exceeding the peripersonal space range (Kolarik, Moore, Zahorik, Cirstea, & Pardhan, 2016). After the echolocation task was completed, we conducted a visual spatial working memory task (Luck & Vogel, 1997; Tsubomi, Fukuda, Watanabe, & Vogel, 2013) to evaluate the partic- ipants’ capacity for visual working memory. 4 i-Perception 10(4) Methods Participants In total, 39 sighted students from Hokkaido University (18–25 years; 17 females) participat- ed in the experiment for monetary compensation or course credit. Three participants were excluded from the analyses due to failure to follow the instructions. One of the remaining 36 was left-handed. In echolocation task, the 36 participants completed the task on the first and second days, and 24 of the 36 (19–25 years; six females) participants completed the follow-up task on the third day. In visual working memory task, in total, the same 24 and 1 participant who did not participate the follow-up echolocation task completed the working memory task. One of the 25 was excluded from the analyses because of a high percentage of error responses (61.1%). All participants reported normal or corrected to normal visual acuity. None of the participants were hearing impaired according to self-report and a hearing test given before the main experiment. During the pretest, the participants were required to detect tones (500–8000 Hz, <20 dB HL) randomly played via headphones (MDR-XB450; Sony Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) in the right and left ears separately. The intensity level of the playing tones was calibrated by a sound level meter (TM-103; Tenmars Electronics Co., Ltd., Taipei, Taiwan) and converted from dB SPL into HL using a conversion factor (ANSI S3.6-2010). None of the participants had prior experience with the echolocation task. This study was approved by the Ethics Board of Hokkaido University, and the participants provided written informed consent prior to the experiment. Apparatus and Stimuli Echolocation task. Figure 1 shows the apparatus used for the detection task during the 3 days. The experiment was conducted in a space separated by a curtain (1.6 m width  4.8 m depth  2.5 m height) in a quiet laboratory. The wall was covered with 2 cm thick Styrofoam board to reduce noise reflection. The ceiling was covered with plasterboard. The ambient sound level in the space was approximately 34 dB as measured using a sound level meter. Target Echolocaon Presence or Press 4 preparaon clicks Absence or 6 key 10 s Unl Response Time Figure 1. The object-detection task. Participants were seated in a chair and produced echolocation cues from a loud speaker by pressing a key on a numeric keypad to identify the presence/absence of a target. Maezawa and Kawahara 5 A table (1.1 m width  2.4 m depth  0.7 m height) was placed in the middle of the space. The top surface of the table was covered with carpet fabric placed on a 3 cm thick Styrofoam board. A loudspeaker (main speaker; SRS-BTX500; Sony Co., Ltd.; 20–20000 Hz), emitting echolocation cues, was mounted on the shorter edge of the table and was connected to a PC/AT-compatible computer (OptiPlex 990; Dell Inc., Round Rock, TX, USA) operating with Linux via a standard audio device (ALC269VB; Realtek Semiconductor Corp., Hsinchu, Taiwan). Another loudspeaker (sub speaker; SRS-BTV5; Sony Co., Ltd.; 20–20000 Hz) was mounted on the right side of the longer edge and was connected to an audio player (iPod Touch; Apple Inc., Cupertino, CA, USA). Two steel bars (2 mm thickness and 90 cm height) were placed vertically on the shorter side of the table and a black roll curtain was mounted on the top to block the participants’ vision. The participant was seated to the rear of the main speaker and the curtain on a height-adjustable chair, facing 3.6 m away from the front wall of the space and 0.85 m away from the nearest wall. Responses were collected via a numeric keypad connected to the computer. To send a signal to the partic- ipant, we used a vibration motor driven by the computer via a microcontroller board (Arduino Uno R3; BCMI US LLC, Boston, MA, USA). The target was a flat aluminum plate (3 mm thickness; 40 cm width 30 cm height) ver- tically mounted on an L-shaped bookend (1 mm thickness; 13 cm width  17 cm height). The position of the target was experimentally manipulated (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm from the participant’s body). The sound cues for echolocation were presented through the main speaker as approximately 6-ms-long artificial clicks generated by a Matlab function as a sinusoidal of exponentially decay- ing 4 kHz by 24-bit resolution and a 96 kHz sampling rate. We chose this function to simulate a waveform for a mouth click because it has been established that a sinusoid amplitude modulated by the exponentially decaying well represents human mouth clicks (Rojas, Hermosilla, Montero, & Espı, 2009; Thaler & Castillo-Serrano, 2016). One click was played through the main speaker at 95 dB when the participant pressed the “5” key on the numeric keypad. Each signal was captured with a microphone (B3 Omnidirectional Lavalier; Countryman Associates, Menlo Park, CA, USA; 20–20000 Hz) and digitized with 32-bit accuracy at a 96-kHz sampling rate using a high-speed USB audio interface (OCTA-CAPTURE; Roland, Shizuoka, Japan). Illustrations of the wave form and power spectrum are shown in Figure 2. Visual spatial working memory task. We used a standard visual spatial working memory task and colored squares as stimuli (Luck & Vogel, 1997; Tsubomi et al., 2013). An example of the stimuli is illustrated in Figure 3. Participants were required to memorize color–location combinations of the sample stimuli and to indicate whether the color of the cued patch in the test stimuli followed the sample stimuli separated by a blank display was identical to the color of the corresponding patch in the sample. The stimuli were displayed on a computer monitor (G2420HD; BenQ Co., Ltd., Haryana, India; driving at a rate of 60 Hz of refresh, 1920  1080 pixels), controlled by custom Matlab code using Psychophysics Toolbox exten- sions (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). The viewing distance was approximately 57 cm. Responses were collected via a computer keyboard. All stimuli and test cues were displayed on a gray background on the monitor. A sample stimulus array consisted of colorful squares (four levels of memory set sizes: three, four, five, or six squares, 1  1 in width and height of the visual angle). Each square was randomly assigned one of a set of nine highly discriminable colors (red, pink, orange, yellow, green, yellow-green, blue, light blue, and white) without replacement and locations from a set of eight possible locations spaced equally 45 along an invisible circle of a radius of 2.9 in visual angle. A square cue (1  1 in width and height in visual angle) indicated one of the sample stimulus locations, so 6 i-Perception 10(4) Figure 2. Waveform of a click produced through the loudspeaker (top half of figure) and the click’s power spectrum (bottom half of figure) for each target distance (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm) and for the target absent conditions. that the vertical halves of the area were filled with two different colors. One of the colors was the same as the sample at the cued location, and the other was a new color that was not presented in a sample array. Participants indicated the color they saw in the sample display. Procedure Echolocation task. All participants completed the echolocation task over 2 days separated between 1 and 8 days (M ¼ 2.83 day, SD ¼ 2.23) and completed the task on the third day after 30 to 137 days (M ¼ 77.3 day, SD ¼ 42.2) from the second day. The participants did not repeat the task 2 or 3 times in the same day. Maezawa and Kawahara 7 Fixaon cross Sample Blank Test Right or Le 1000 ms 200 ms 1000 ms Unl response Time Figure 3. Schematic diagram of the visual spatial working memory task. Participants viewed a sample display for 200 ms followed by a test display. The participants were required to identify a color binding a location of a prior sample stimulus by providing a right or left response. The experiment was a 3  4  2 factorial design with three within-subjects factors of day (1, 2, or 3), target distance (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm), and target presence (presence or absence). Each condition was randomly assigned for each trial. A session consisted of 32 practice trials followed by 120 test trials per day. One participant quit the test at 80 trials due to a technical error. The trials were completed in approximately 70 minutes per day, including 15-minute practice trials and one 5-minute break. All participants were visually impeded by a close-fitting eye mask at the beginning of the trials. The participants fixed their head position at approximately 10 to 20 cm above the center of, and 5 to 15 cm behind, the main speaker; thus, their head was not hidden by it. We did not use a chinrest to avoid discomfort to the participants caused by a long period of physical restraint. Each participant held the vibration motor by their nondominant hand or placed it on their thighs. To mask sounds related to the placement of the target, the black roll curtain obstructed the target from the participant and the subspeaker played 80 dB of pink noise until the target was placed on the table (or was not placed, in the target-absent trials). After the target was ready, the participant was prompted by the vibration motor signal and allowed to produce clicks for 10 seconds by pressing the “5” key on the numeric keypad with their dominant hand. Another vibration signaled the end of the 10 seconds of the trial, and the participant pressed the “6” key on the keypad for presence or the “4” key for absence. The responses and number of clicks were recorded by the computer. We calculated the dis- criminability index d (Green & Swets, 1974) as the difference between the z-transforms of the hit and false alarm rate; d ¼ z (hit rate) – z (false alarm rate). The hit and false alarm rates were converted from 1 to 1 – 1/(2N) and from 0 to 1/(2N)(Macmillan & Creelman, 1991). Participants received feedback by vibration upon providing a correct answer for target presence/absence during the practice trials to familiarize themselves with detection task pro- cedure. However, they received no such feedback during the experimental trials. Visual spatial working memory task. The visual working memory capacity task was conducted on the first or third day of the echolocation task depending on each participant’s availability. Each participant performed a total of 216 trials consisting of 54 trials per condition (three, four, five, or six of the memory set size). As shown in Figure 2, each trial began with the presentation of a central fixation cross (0.2  0.2 ) for 1,000 ms, followed by the sample stimulus array for 200 ms. After a blank period of 1,000 ms, the test cue remained on the screen until the participant responded. The participants indicated whether the cued color was 8 i-Perception 10(4) on the right or left by pressing the keys for “i” (for right) or “e” (for left) responses. We computed the estimating capacity score of visual working memory with the common formula: K ¼ S (P – 50)/50, where K is the visual capacity, S is the memory set size, and P is percentage correct (Cowan, 2001). The formula represents the individual’s ability to hold K items from a sample array of S items in their working memory. Results Detection Performance Across 2 Days The d values obtained from individual participants (n ¼ 36) were averaged separately for each target distance. Figure 4(a) shows the detection performance of the echolocation task on the first and second days. To test the improvement in detection performance across the 2 days, we subjected the d scores to a 2 (Day 1 and 2)  4 (Target distance 20, 30, 40, and (a) (b) (d) (c) Figure 4. (a) Mean d score for Day 1 or 2 as a function of target distance (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm). Error bars represent SEMs. (b) Box-and-whisker plots showing the median, interquartile, and range with 1.5  interquartile range whiskers for the d score averaged across the 2 test days. (c) Mean number of clicks for Day 1 or 2 as a function of target distance (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm). Error bars represent SEMs. (d) Plots of 0 0 the increase in d score averaged across the 2 test days as a function of d on Day 1. The solid line represents the regression line of the plot. Maezawa and Kawahara 9 50 cm) repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). The ANOVA revealed significant main effect of day, F(1, 35) ¼ 9.22, p ¼ .005, g ¼ .21, and target distance, F(3, 105) ¼ 17.27, 2 2 p< .001, g ¼ .33. Their interaction was not significant, F(3, 105) ¼ 0.80, p ¼ .494, g ¼.02. p p Multiple comparisons by Holm’s method revealed that the d score decreased from 20 to 30 cm, t(35) ¼ 3.92, p ¼ .002, r ¼ .55; from 20 to 40 cm, t(35) ¼ 6.54, p< .001, r ¼.74; and from 20 to 50 cm, t(35) ¼ 5.49, p< .001, r ¼ .68. The d score averaged across both days fluctuated substantially within participants (box- plot, Figure 4(b)); the means (SD) were 1.82 (0.87), 1.44 (0.85), 1.20 (0.84), and 1.20 (0.81) when the target was located at 20, 30, 40, and 50 cm, respectively. These scores were signif- icantly higher than 0 when the target was placed at 20 cm, t(35) ¼ 12.59, p< .001, r ¼ .91; at 30 cm, t(35) ¼ 10.11, p< .001, r ¼ .86; at 40 cm, t(35) ¼ 8.62, p< .001, r ¼ .82; and at 50 cm, t(35) ¼ 8.87, p< .001, r ¼ .83. We also investigated the ceiling effect for improvement in detection performance. Participants who performed well on Day 1 improved less than those whose initial perfor- mance was poorer. The increase in detection performance was calculated by subtracting the 0 0 0 d of Day 1 from the d of Day 2. Figure 4(c) shows a plot of the increase in d score as a function of d on Day 1. We computed Pearson’s correlation coefficients, and the results showed a significant negative correlation between the observed increase in performance and d score on Day 1, r(36)¼.50, p ¼ .002, indicating that high-performing individuals improved their performance less across the 2 days than did low-performance individuals. We also assessed the effects of increasing in the number of clicks during the detection task. The participants’ number of emission sounds per trial are shown in Figure 4(d), as a function of target distances during the target trials. The mean number of clicks per trial was subjected to a repeated-measures ANOVA with a 2 (Day 1 and 2)  4 (Target distance 20, 30, 40, and 50 cm) design. The results revealed a significant effect of target distance, F(3, 105) ¼ 2.96, 2 2 p ¼ .036, g ¼.08; while a no significant effect of day, F(1, 35) ¼ 0.68, p ¼ .414, g ¼ .02, and p p their interaction, F(3, 105) ¼ 1.54, p ¼.210, g ¼.04. There was no significant correlation between the mean number of clicks per trial averaged across the distances and the d score, r(36) ¼.09, p ¼ .607; this indicated that an increase in the number of clicks did not improve or impair detection performance. Detection Performance on the Third Day The d on Day 3 obtained from individual participants were averaged separately for each target distance. Figure 5 shows the detection performance of the echolocation task on Days 2 and 3 in the 24 samples who completed the follow-up echolocation task on the third day. To examine maintaining improved detection performance over a month, we subjected the d scores to a repeated-measures ANOVA with a 3 (Day 2 and 3)  4 (Target distance 20, 30, 40, and 50 cm) design. The ANOVA revealed significant main effect of target distance, F(3, 69) ¼ 16.34, p< .001, g ¼ .42; while no significant effect of day, F(1, 23) ¼ 0.56, 2 2 p ¼ .461, g ¼ .02, and their interaction, F(6, 69) ¼ 1.50, p ¼ .222, g ¼.06, which indicated p p that improved detection performance was retained for at least 1 month. Visual Working Memory Capacity We examined the association between visual working memory capacity and object detection performance by echolocation, separately for each experimental day (Days 1–3). We also examined whether improved detection performance across 2 days was modulated by working memory capacity. Plots of detection performance scores as a function of the capacity K are 10 i-Perception 10(4) Figure 5. Mean d score of the 24 samples for Day 2 or 3 as a function of target distance (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm). Error bars represent SEMs. shown in Figure 6. The mean performance (K) on the task was 2.38 (95% CI [2.10, 2.65], SD ¼ 0.65). The results revealed a significant positive correlation between the visual working memory capacity score and the d score on Day 1, r(24) ¼ .48, p ¼ .017; and on Day 3, r(23) ¼ .49, p ¼ .018; there was no significant correlation between visual working memory capacity score and either the detection performance on Day 2, r(24) ¼ .30, p ¼ .155, or the increase in performance, r(24)¼.20, p ¼ .354. Supplementary Experiment To examine whether visual working memory capacity modulated detection performance on echolocation tasks, we further measured each individual’s visual cognitive performance using a matrix test (Cornoldi, Cortesi, & Preti, 1991), the score of which can be taken as a measure of spatial working memory capacity. In this test, participants were asked to follow an imaginary pathway through two-dimensional matrices. Twelve of the 36 original participants performed the matrix test. In addition, five students who did not perform the initial echolocation task participated in the same echolocation task for only 1 day as well as in the matrix test. Total of 17 participants (18–26 years; 7 females) completed the matrix test on the second day of the experiment or the earlier, depending on each participant’s availability. We examined the cor- relation between the performances on the matrix test and on echolocation task on the first day. Figure 7 shows a schematic diagram of a trial of the matrix test. All stimuli were displayed on a black background on an LCD monitor (XL2411T; BenQ; 100 Hz refresh rate, 1,920 1,080 pixels). Participants were required to memorize a target location displayed in acell of an 11  11 matrix, subtending 24  24 in width and height of the visual angle. Their task was to mentally maneuver the target according to visual directional cues. Each trial began with the presentation of a central fixation cross (0.2  0.2 ) for 1,000 ms, followed by the target stimulus in the matrix for 5,000 ms. The target was a red square, the initial location of which was randomly selected from the matrix. After a blank screen was shown for 1,000 ms, arrow cues (9  9 ) were sequentially presented in the center of the screen. The set size of the series of the cues (4, 6, or 8) and direction (right, left, up, or down) were assigned Maezawa and Kawahara 11 (a) (b) (c) (d) (e) Figure 6. Behavioral results of the visual working memory task. (a) Mean visual working memory capacity (K) as a function of memory set size (3, 4, 5, or 6). Error bars represent SEMs. (b–d) Correlations between visual working memory capacity and d for Day 1, Day 2, or Day 3. It should be noted that several points overlapped each other. The solid line represents the regression line of the plot. (e) Correlation between visual working memory and increase in d across the 2 test days. randomly. Each directional cue was presented for 1,000 ms, and the cues were separated by a blank screen presented for 1,000 ms. After the presentation of a series of directional cues, the matrix was displayed on its own. Participants indicated the final target destination by clicking a cell in the matrix. Each participant performed a total of 30 trials consisting of 10 trials of each set size of the sequence length. The proportions of correct responses obtained from individual participants were averaged across the sequence length. The mean correct proportion was 0.82 (95% CI [0.76, 0.88], SD ¼ 0.11). One participant was removed from the analysis as an outlier, indicated by a Cook’s distance exceeding 0.5. We examined the association between the mean proportion of correct responses and echolocation performance on the first day (n¼ 16). The plot of the d scores on Day 1 against the mean proportion of correct responses is shown in Figure 8. We computed Pearson’s correlation coefficients, and the results revealed a significant positive correlation between the matrix test score and the d score on Day 1, r(16)¼ .50, p¼ .049. Discussion We examined the association between echolocation performance in sighted individuals and individual factors, such as visual working memory capacity. We found that the participants’ visual working memory capacity was positively correlated with detection performance, 12 i-Perception 10(4) Direconal cues …. 1000 ms 1000 ms 1000 ms 1000 ms Fixaon cross Target Blank Test Direconal cues 1000 ms 5000 ms 1000 ms Unl Response Time Figure 7. Schematic diagram of the matrix test. Participants followed an imaginary pathway through two- dimensional matrices (11  11; the example shows a scaled down 5  5 grid). The participants were required to identify the final destination of the target. (a) (b) Figure 8. Behavioral results of the matrix test. (a) Mean proportion of correct responses as a function of memory set size (4, 6, or 8). Error bars represent SEMs. (b) Correlation between the proportion of correct response and echolocation performance (d ) on Day 1. The solid line represents the regression line of the plot. Maezawa and Kawahara 13 expect on the second day. Detection performance did not decline over several months. In other words, we found that the improved detection achieved on the second day was maintained for at least 30 to 137 days. Interestingly, detection performance improved over the localization task repeated for 120 trials (approximately 50 minutes) per day without any feedback, although participants did not perform as well as subjects in a previous study that included feedback on every trial (Schenkman & Nilsson, 2010). This indicates that sighted participants can improve their performance following a relatively small amount of repetitive training during the task. However, we found individual differences in detection performance. Specifically, individuals’ performances on target detection tests fluctuated substantially, reflected by the large stan- dard deviations and range values of the data. The present results extend the findings of Ekkel et al. (2017), in that we assessed spatial working memory capacity; participants required to memorize visual objects being actively used during visual spatial processing while not involving auditory verbal processing. Our results show a significant correlation between visual working memory capacity and detection performance during echolocation, except for performance on the second day, after performance had improved. The correlation between visual working memory capacity and performance on the second day was not significant, and the coefficient value decreased compared to the first day. This lack of a correlation on the second day might be due to the improvement in performance. Individuals who performed well on the first day of the tests improved less than those who initially had low scores, and thus the regression line of detec- tion performance on working memory capacity flattened out on the second day, decreasing the correlation coefficient value. Our results were inconsistent with those of by Ekkel et al. (2017), in that we did not find a significant correlation between working memory capacity and the increase in detection per- formance across the 2 days. This difference seems reasonable because of the ceiling effect for enhancement of echolocation ability. Specifically, because there was a positive correlation between echolocation performance and working memory capacity on the first day, individ- uals who had large memory capacities were expected to show a smaller increase in detection performance across the 2 days. In fact, the results indicated a negative (but nonsignificant) correlation (r¼.20). Another possible explanation is that the study by Ekkel et al. (2017) assessed participants’ working memory capacity in the context of the central executive com- ponent, rather than a spatial subcomponent, whereas this study measured the working memory capacity as a reflection of spatial aspects. Also, Ekkel et al. assessed performance using a size discrimination task, whereas this study used a different type of task, involving object detection (Schenkman & Nilsson, 2010). Additional research is needed to determine the influence of types of echolocation and working memory tasks on correlations between working memory capacity and echolocation ability. In summary, visual working memory capacity predicts object detection performance on an echolocation task. The present finding adds to the literature in that the correlation between echolocation ability, rather than the degree of improvement in echolocation due to training, and visual working memory capacity has never been examined. The involvement of vision during echolocation processing has been suggested by Thaler et al. (2014), who indicated that echolocation involves spatial cognitive processing (e.g., mental imagery). The visual area may not be involved during other auditory control tasks (e.g., change in the sound location; Thaler & Foresteire, 2017). The present results support these findings regarding visual involvement in echolocation and the spatial component of working memory by sighted participants. Furthermore, the findings suggest that visual working memory capacity is predictive of higher performance of the detection task for sighted participants. 14 i-Perception 10(4) The question of how much training is necessary to learn echolocation remains unresolved. To clarify this issue, future studies should manipulate the amount of echolocation task training to determine a sufficient amount of training as well as to investigate how perfor- mance improves. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This work was supported by Graduate Grant Program of Graduate School of Letters, Hokkaido University, Japan (to T. M.) and by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (17H02648) (to J. I. K.). ORCID iD Tomoki Maezawa https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9077-3132 References Au, W. W. L., Branstetter, B. K., Benoit-Bird, K. J., & Kastelein, R. A. (2009). Acoustic basis for fish prey discrimination by echolocating dolphins and porpoises. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126, 460–467. doi:10.1121/1.3147497 Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422 Barclay, R. M. R., & Brigham, R. M. (1994). Constraints on optimal foraging: Am, field test of prey discrimination by echolocating insectivorous bats. Animal Behaviour, 48, 1013–1021. doi:10.1006/ anbe.1994.1334 Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436. doi:10.1163/ 156856897X00357 Carlson-Smith, C., & Weiner, W. R. (1996). The auditory skills necessary for echolocation: A new explanation. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 90, 21–35. Cornoldi, C., Cortesi, A., & Preti, D. (1991). Individual differences in the capacity limitations of visuospatial short-term memory: Research on sighted and totally congenitally blind people. Memory & Cognition, 19, 459–468. doi:10.3758/BF03199569 Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003922 Ekkel, M. R., van Lier, R., & Steenbergen, B. (2017). Learning to echolocate in sighted people: A correlational study on attention, working memory and spatial abilities. Experimental Brain Research, 235, 809–818. doi:10.1007/s00221-016-4833-z Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1974). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Oxford, England: Robert E. Krieger. Gronwall, D. M. (1977). Paced auditory serial-addition task: A measure of recovery from concussion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 367–373. doi:10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367 Kolarik, A. J., Cirstea, S., Pardhan, S., & Moore, B. C. J. (2014). A summary of research investigating echolocation abilities of blind and sighted humans. Hearing Research, 310, 60–68. doi:10.1016/j. heares.2014.01.010 Maezawa and Kawahara 15 Kolarik, A. J., Moore, B. C. J., Zahorik, P., Cirstea, S., & Pardhan, S. (2016). Auditory distance perception in humans: A review of cues, development, neuronal bases, and effects of sensory loss. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78, 373–395. doi:0.3758/s13414-015-1015-1 Kolarik, A. J., Scarfe, A. C., Moore, B. C. J., & Pardhan, S. (2016). An assessment of auditory-guided locomotion in an obstacle circumvention task. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 1725–1735. doi:10.1007/s00221-016-4567-y Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14, 389–433. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(05)80012-1 Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., McHugh, B., & Giudice, N. A. (2012). Spatial working memory for locations specified by vision and audition: Testing the amodality hypothesis. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1260–1267. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0311-2 Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunc- tions. Nature, 390, 279–281. doi:10.1038/36846 Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (2013). Visual working memory capacity: From psychophysics and neuro- biology to individual differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 391–400. doi:10.1016/j. tics.2013.06.006 Luria, R., & Vogel, E. K. (2011). Visual search demands dictate reliance on working memory storage. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 6199–6207. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6453-10.2011 Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Mance, I., & Vogel, E. K. (2013). Visual working memory. WIREs Cognitive Science, 4, 179–190. doi:10.1002/wcs.1219 Moossavi, A., Mehrkian, S., Lotfi, Y., Faghihzadeh, S., & Sajedi, H. (2014). The relation between working memory capacity and auditory lateralization in children with auditory processing disorders. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 78, 1981–1986. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2014.09.003 Neuweiler, G. (1990). Auditory adaptations for prey capture in echolocating bats. Physiological Reviews, 70, 615–641. doi:10.1152/physrev.1990.70.3.615 Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442. doi:10.1163/156856897X00366 Rice, C. E. (1967). Human echo perception. Science, 155, 656–664. doi:10.1126/science.155.3763.656 Rice, C. E., & Feinstein, S. H. (1965). Sonar system of the blind: Size discrimination. Science, 148, 1107–1108. doi:10.1126/science.148.3673.1107 Ricker, T. J., AuBuchon, A. M., & Cowan, N. (2010). Working memory. WIREs Cognitive Science, 1, 573–585. doi:10.1002/wcs.50 Rojas, J. A. M., Hermosilla, J. A., Montero, R. S., & Espı, P. L. L. (2009). Physical analysis of several organic signals for human echolocation: Oral vacuum pulses. Acta Acustica United With Acustica, 95, 325–330. doi:10.3813/AAA.918155 Schenkman, B. N., & Nilsson, M. E. (2010). Human echolocation: Blind and sighted persons’ ability to detect sounds recorded in the presence of a reflecting object. Perception, 39, 483–501. doi:10.1068/p6473 Serino, A., Ciaramelli, E., Santantonio, A. D., Malagu,  S., Servadei, F., & La` davas, E. (2007). A pilot study for rehabilitation of central executive deficits after traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 21, 11–19. doi:10.1080/02699050601151811 Tao, Q., Chan, C. C. H., Luo, Y.-j., Li, J.-j., Ting, K.-h., Wang, J., & Lee, T. M. C. (2015). How does experience modulate auditory spatial processing in individuals with blindness? Brain Topography, 28, 506–519. doi:10.1007/s10548-013-0339-1 Teng, S., Puri, A., & Whitney, D. (2012). Ultrafine spatial acuity of blind expert human echolocators. Experimental Brain Research, 216, 483–488. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2951-1 Teng, S., & Whitney, D. (2011). The acuity of echolocation: Spatial resolution in the sighted compared to expert performance. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 105, 20–32. doi:10.1177/ 0145482X1110500103 16 i-Perception 10(4) Thaler, L., & Castillo-Serrano, J. (2016). People’s ability to detect objects using click-based echoloca- tion: A direct comparison between mouth-clicks and clicks made by a loudspeaker. PLoS One, 11, e0154868. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0154868 Thaler, L., & Foresteire, D. (2017). Visual sensory stimulation interferes with people’s ability to echolocate object size. Scientific Reports, 7, 13069. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-12967-3 Thaler, L., Wilson, R. C., & Gee, B. K. (2014). Correlation between vividness of visual imagery and echolocation ability in sighted, echo-naive people. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 1915–1925. doi:10.1007/s00221-014-3883-3 Tonelli, A., Brayda, L., & Gori, M. (2016). Depth echolocation learnt by novice sighted people. PLoS One, 11, e0156654. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0156654 Tonelli, A., Campus, C., & Brayda, L. (2018). How body motion influences echolocation while walking. Scientific Reports, 8, 15704. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-34074-7 Tsubomi, H., Fukuda, K., Watanabe, K., & Vogel, E. K. (2013). Neural limits to representing objects still within view. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 8257–8263. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5348-12.2013 Unsworth, N., Fukuda, K., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2014). Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval. Cognitive Psychology, 71, 1–26. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.01.003 Vogel, E. K., McCullough, A. W., & Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature, 438, 500–503. doi:10.1038/nature04171 Worchel, P., Mauney, J., & Andrew, J. G. (1950). The perception of obstacles by the blind. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 746–751. doi:10.1037/h0060950 Zahorik, P., Bangayan, P., Sundareswaran, V., Wang, K., & Tam, C. (2006). Perceptual recalibration in human sound localization: Learning to remediate front-back reversals. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 343–359. doi:10.1121/1.2208429 How to cite this article Maezawa, T., & Kawahara, J. I. (2019). Effects of visual working memory on individual differences in echolocation performance in sighted participants. i-Perception, 10(4), 1–16. doi: 10.1177/ http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png i-Perception SAGE

Effects of Visual Working Memory on Individual Differences in Echolocation Performance in Sighted Participants:

i-Perception , Volume 10 (4): 1 – Aug 29, 2019

Loading next page...
 
/lp/sage/effects-of-visual-working-memory-on-individual-differences-in-9v0uQJXDJ0

References (45)

Publisher
SAGE
Copyright
Copyright © 2022 by SAGE Publications Ltd unless otherwise noted. Manuscript content on this site is licensed under Creative Commons Licenses
ISSN
2041-6695
eISSN
2041-6695
DOI
10.1177/2041669519872223
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

Echolocation performance differs widely among individuals. This study examined a possible factor that may explain this variation, namely, visual working memory, which is a subcomponent of spatial working memory. Sighted participants performed an object-detection task consisting of initial testing on 2 separate days (up to 8 days apart) with follow-up testing on a third day (up to 1 month after the second day of testing) while manipulating the target distance from 20 to 50 cm. Participants performed two types of visual spatial working memory tasks, one of which required them to memorize color–location combinations and the other, an imaginary pathway. The participants’ performance on the object-detection task generally improved in the first 2 days, but there were substantial individual differences in detection ability. A positive correlation was observed between performance on these tasks and visual working memory capacity, except on the second day, after detection ability had improved. These findings suggest that factors contrib- uting to echolocation skill are related to nonauditory factors in a sighted group. Keywords echolocation, perception, visual spatial working memory Date received: 11 March 2019; accepted: 1 August 2019 Introduction Echolocation is a method to localize objects and acquire object features (e.g., distance, size, shape, and surface of material) based on the reflection of sound. Echolocation ability has been Corresponding author: Tomoki Maezawa, Hokkaido University, N10W7, Kita, Sapporo 060-0810, Japan. Email: porpoise@let.hokudai.ac.jp Creative Commons CC BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage). 2 i-Perception 10(4) extensively acknowledged in nonhuman species, such as odontocetes (e.g., Au, Branstetter, Benoit-Bird, & Kastelein, 2009) and some bats (e.g., Barclay & Brigham, 1994; Neuweiler, 1990). Similarly, humans with normal hearing can use echolocation (Rice & Feinstein, 1965; Schenkman & Nilsson, 2010). Particularly, the sighted population can echolocate (Teng & Whitney, 2011) using several types of echo-acoustic sounds, such as oral clicks and artificial click noises via loud speakers (Thaler & Castillo-Serrano, 2016; Tonelli, Brayda, & Gori, 2016; Tonelli, Campus, & Brayda, 2018), as well as auditory substitution to enhance spatial awareness for successful navigation (Kolarik, Scarfe, Moore, & Pardhan, 2016). Echolocation skill can be acquired with suitable training, although most sighted groups are less sensitive to the cues available for echolocation (Kolarik, Cirstea, Pardhan, & Moore, 2014). Rapid improvements in echolocation performance occur in sighted populations fol- lowing only a few training sessions (Teng & Whitney, 2011). These findings indicate the need for repetitive training to acquire echolocation skill. However, large individual differences in echolocation performance and the degree of improvement during echolocation training may complicate the establishment of systematic echolocation training protocols (e.g., Ekkel, van Lier, & Steenbergen, 2017), and some individuals are unable to acquire echolocation skills (e.g., Worchel, Mauney, & Andrew, 1950). Thus, a better understanding of the factors contributing to these individual differences is necessary to aid in the training of echolocation skills. A plausible explanation for the individual differences is that echolocation performance is affected by variation in auditory sensitivity of the hearing system because performance is based on detecting and discriminating auditory echoic cues. A set of studies has supported this relationship (e.g., Carlson-Smith & Wiener, 1996; Schenkman & Nilsson, 2010). Specifically, experiences with echoic information, auditory perceptual learning, and task feedback are associated with echolocation performance (e.g., Teng & Whitney, 2011; Teng, Puri, & Whitney, 2012). However, the ability to discriminate echoic cues is not the only factor that explains the individual variability (Rice, 1967) because sighted populations, in particular, do not rely on echoic cues relative to blind populations. Thus, other factors are involved. In this study, we argue that working memory capacity plays a role in echolocation per- formance. Working memory is the temporary storage and workspace to manipulate infor- mation for ongoing tasks (e.g., Baddeley, 2012; Ricker, AuBuchon, & Cowan, 2010), and functions in conjunction with a subsidiary slave system of the attentional control system, known as the central executive (Baddeley, 2000). The relevance of working memory in audi- tory cognitive processing has been acknowledged (e.g., Moossavi, Mehrkian, Lotfi, Faghihzadeh, & Sajedi, 2014). In fact, the degree of improvement in echolocation perfor- mance positively correlates with a test score for the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT; Gronwall, 1977), reflecting working memory and divided attention (Ekkel et al., 2017), in conjunction with auditory verbal aspects. The PASAT has been used as a measure of impairments in the central executive of working memory (e.g., Serino et al., 2007); thus, the results of Ekkel et al. (2017) support the notion that working memory capacity modulates echolocation performance in size discrimination. However, it should be noted that the indi- vidual differences in the ability to use echolocation was beyond the scope of Ekkel et al.’s study. Rather, their focus was on the differences in improvements in echolocation perfor- mance due to training. Moreover, even though the central executive component would con- tribute to the learning of size discrimination (Ekkel et al., 2017), the processing of echolocation relates to spatial perception; thus, the process should involve the spatial com- ponent of working memory. Thus, further investigation is required to examine whether echolocation performance is linked to the capacity of spatial working memory, rather than central executive functions. Maezawa and Kawahara 3 Visual working memory (Luck & Vogel, 2013; Mance & Vogel, 2013) is a subsidiary system of working memory, contributing to maintenance of visual information to serve the needs of ongoing tasks. The concept of the spatial subcomponent of working memory has played a central role in explaining individual differences in cognitive performance in the spatial domain (Kyllonen & Christal, 1990). Given that high visual working memory capac- ity is predictive of higher performance on visual tasks, such as visual search, localization and detection of changes (e.g., Luria & Vogel, 2011; Unsworth, Fukuda, Awh, & Vogel, 2014), visual working memory should predict individual differences in cognitive performance involving spatial processes. Importantly, visual working memory capacity predicts individ- uals’ filtering efficiency, so that in individuals with high visual working memory capacity, irrelevant distractor-related information is efficiently excluded from storage in memory, thus preserving the capacity for purposeful use (Vogel, McCullough, & Machizawa, 2005). Although the concept of visual working memory has been traditionally related to active maintenance of visual representations, recent studies on working memory have extended the concept to spatial processing of images in auditory modality tasks (Loomis, Klatzky, McHugh, & Giudice, 2012). We hypothesized that visual processing and experience would contribute to the representation of mental images and aid echolocation in sighted subjects. In fact, much effort has been expended on characterizing the relationship between echolo- cation and visual cognitive processing (Tao et al., 2015; Thaler & Foresteire, 2017; Thaler, Wilson, & Gee, 2014). Thus, we expected that sighted individuals with superior visual work- ing memory capacity would perform better in auditory spatial tasks, such as target search and change detection tasks. In other words, individuals with high working memory capacity should be able to detect a target object from distractor noise due to high-filtering efficiency. Thus, the aim of this study was to examine the association between echolocation perfor- mance in sighted individuals and how individual factors of visual working memory capacity contribute to performance. Specifically, we examined whether visual working memory capac- ity correlates with object detection performance in echolocation tasks. We also investigated whether improved detection performance is retained over a month, by asking participants to perform the same echolocation tasks 3 times over that time periods. Maintaining improved echolocation ability after training presents a challenge for echolocation learning. In one study (Zahorik, Bangayan, Sundareswaran, Wang, & Tam, 2006) that did not use an echo- location task, improved ability in sound localization persisted for at least 4 months. Similar retention of echolocation improvement was expected in this study. We designed an object-detection task based on Schenkman and Nilsson (2010), whose participants determined the presence or absence of a target in front of them using echolo- cation, while the distance to the target was manipulated. The experiment lasted 120 trials (approximately 50 minutes) per day across 2 days and the amount of training during the task was greatly reduced from that used by Schenkman and Nilsson (2010), who used 56 trials per session across 36 sessions (2.5–3 hours) with feedback on every trial. We also omitted the feedback. Specifically, our experiment consisted of initial testing on 2 separate days (up to 8 days apart) with a follow-up testing on a third day (up to 1 month apart). Two sighted participants in our pilot study showed a chance level in detecting a target placed more than 50 cm away; thus, we chose a target distance of 20 to 50 cm. This range was shorter than the previous study (Schenkman & Nilsson, 2010) that covered from 50 to 500 cm exceeding the peripersonal space range (Kolarik, Moore, Zahorik, Cirstea, & Pardhan, 2016). After the echolocation task was completed, we conducted a visual spatial working memory task (Luck & Vogel, 1997; Tsubomi, Fukuda, Watanabe, & Vogel, 2013) to evaluate the partic- ipants’ capacity for visual working memory. 4 i-Perception 10(4) Methods Participants In total, 39 sighted students from Hokkaido University (18–25 years; 17 females) participat- ed in the experiment for monetary compensation or course credit. Three participants were excluded from the analyses due to failure to follow the instructions. One of the remaining 36 was left-handed. In echolocation task, the 36 participants completed the task on the first and second days, and 24 of the 36 (19–25 years; six females) participants completed the follow-up task on the third day. In visual working memory task, in total, the same 24 and 1 participant who did not participate the follow-up echolocation task completed the working memory task. One of the 25 was excluded from the analyses because of a high percentage of error responses (61.1%). All participants reported normal or corrected to normal visual acuity. None of the participants were hearing impaired according to self-report and a hearing test given before the main experiment. During the pretest, the participants were required to detect tones (500–8000 Hz, <20 dB HL) randomly played via headphones (MDR-XB450; Sony Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) in the right and left ears separately. The intensity level of the playing tones was calibrated by a sound level meter (TM-103; Tenmars Electronics Co., Ltd., Taipei, Taiwan) and converted from dB SPL into HL using a conversion factor (ANSI S3.6-2010). None of the participants had prior experience with the echolocation task. This study was approved by the Ethics Board of Hokkaido University, and the participants provided written informed consent prior to the experiment. Apparatus and Stimuli Echolocation task. Figure 1 shows the apparatus used for the detection task during the 3 days. The experiment was conducted in a space separated by a curtain (1.6 m width  4.8 m depth  2.5 m height) in a quiet laboratory. The wall was covered with 2 cm thick Styrofoam board to reduce noise reflection. The ceiling was covered with plasterboard. The ambient sound level in the space was approximately 34 dB as measured using a sound level meter. Target Echolocaon Presence or Press 4 preparaon clicks Absence or 6 key 10 s Unl Response Time Figure 1. The object-detection task. Participants were seated in a chair and produced echolocation cues from a loud speaker by pressing a key on a numeric keypad to identify the presence/absence of a target. Maezawa and Kawahara 5 A table (1.1 m width  2.4 m depth  0.7 m height) was placed in the middle of the space. The top surface of the table was covered with carpet fabric placed on a 3 cm thick Styrofoam board. A loudspeaker (main speaker; SRS-BTX500; Sony Co., Ltd.; 20–20000 Hz), emitting echolocation cues, was mounted on the shorter edge of the table and was connected to a PC/AT-compatible computer (OptiPlex 990; Dell Inc., Round Rock, TX, USA) operating with Linux via a standard audio device (ALC269VB; Realtek Semiconductor Corp., Hsinchu, Taiwan). Another loudspeaker (sub speaker; SRS-BTV5; Sony Co., Ltd.; 20–20000 Hz) was mounted on the right side of the longer edge and was connected to an audio player (iPod Touch; Apple Inc., Cupertino, CA, USA). Two steel bars (2 mm thickness and 90 cm height) were placed vertically on the shorter side of the table and a black roll curtain was mounted on the top to block the participants’ vision. The participant was seated to the rear of the main speaker and the curtain on a height-adjustable chair, facing 3.6 m away from the front wall of the space and 0.85 m away from the nearest wall. Responses were collected via a numeric keypad connected to the computer. To send a signal to the partic- ipant, we used a vibration motor driven by the computer via a microcontroller board (Arduino Uno R3; BCMI US LLC, Boston, MA, USA). The target was a flat aluminum plate (3 mm thickness; 40 cm width 30 cm height) ver- tically mounted on an L-shaped bookend (1 mm thickness; 13 cm width  17 cm height). The position of the target was experimentally manipulated (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm from the participant’s body). The sound cues for echolocation were presented through the main speaker as approximately 6-ms-long artificial clicks generated by a Matlab function as a sinusoidal of exponentially decay- ing 4 kHz by 24-bit resolution and a 96 kHz sampling rate. We chose this function to simulate a waveform for a mouth click because it has been established that a sinusoid amplitude modulated by the exponentially decaying well represents human mouth clicks (Rojas, Hermosilla, Montero, & Espı, 2009; Thaler & Castillo-Serrano, 2016). One click was played through the main speaker at 95 dB when the participant pressed the “5” key on the numeric keypad. Each signal was captured with a microphone (B3 Omnidirectional Lavalier; Countryman Associates, Menlo Park, CA, USA; 20–20000 Hz) and digitized with 32-bit accuracy at a 96-kHz sampling rate using a high-speed USB audio interface (OCTA-CAPTURE; Roland, Shizuoka, Japan). Illustrations of the wave form and power spectrum are shown in Figure 2. Visual spatial working memory task. We used a standard visual spatial working memory task and colored squares as stimuli (Luck & Vogel, 1997; Tsubomi et al., 2013). An example of the stimuli is illustrated in Figure 3. Participants were required to memorize color–location combinations of the sample stimuli and to indicate whether the color of the cued patch in the test stimuli followed the sample stimuli separated by a blank display was identical to the color of the corresponding patch in the sample. The stimuli were displayed on a computer monitor (G2420HD; BenQ Co., Ltd., Haryana, India; driving at a rate of 60 Hz of refresh, 1920  1080 pixels), controlled by custom Matlab code using Psychophysics Toolbox exten- sions (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). The viewing distance was approximately 57 cm. Responses were collected via a computer keyboard. All stimuli and test cues were displayed on a gray background on the monitor. A sample stimulus array consisted of colorful squares (four levels of memory set sizes: three, four, five, or six squares, 1  1 in width and height of the visual angle). Each square was randomly assigned one of a set of nine highly discriminable colors (red, pink, orange, yellow, green, yellow-green, blue, light blue, and white) without replacement and locations from a set of eight possible locations spaced equally 45 along an invisible circle of a radius of 2.9 in visual angle. A square cue (1  1 in width and height in visual angle) indicated one of the sample stimulus locations, so 6 i-Perception 10(4) Figure 2. Waveform of a click produced through the loudspeaker (top half of figure) and the click’s power spectrum (bottom half of figure) for each target distance (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm) and for the target absent conditions. that the vertical halves of the area were filled with two different colors. One of the colors was the same as the sample at the cued location, and the other was a new color that was not presented in a sample array. Participants indicated the color they saw in the sample display. Procedure Echolocation task. All participants completed the echolocation task over 2 days separated between 1 and 8 days (M ¼ 2.83 day, SD ¼ 2.23) and completed the task on the third day after 30 to 137 days (M ¼ 77.3 day, SD ¼ 42.2) from the second day. The participants did not repeat the task 2 or 3 times in the same day. Maezawa and Kawahara 7 Fixaon cross Sample Blank Test Right or Le 1000 ms 200 ms 1000 ms Unl response Time Figure 3. Schematic diagram of the visual spatial working memory task. Participants viewed a sample display for 200 ms followed by a test display. The participants were required to identify a color binding a location of a prior sample stimulus by providing a right or left response. The experiment was a 3  4  2 factorial design with three within-subjects factors of day (1, 2, or 3), target distance (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm), and target presence (presence or absence). Each condition was randomly assigned for each trial. A session consisted of 32 practice trials followed by 120 test trials per day. One participant quit the test at 80 trials due to a technical error. The trials were completed in approximately 70 minutes per day, including 15-minute practice trials and one 5-minute break. All participants were visually impeded by a close-fitting eye mask at the beginning of the trials. The participants fixed their head position at approximately 10 to 20 cm above the center of, and 5 to 15 cm behind, the main speaker; thus, their head was not hidden by it. We did not use a chinrest to avoid discomfort to the participants caused by a long period of physical restraint. Each participant held the vibration motor by their nondominant hand or placed it on their thighs. To mask sounds related to the placement of the target, the black roll curtain obstructed the target from the participant and the subspeaker played 80 dB of pink noise until the target was placed on the table (or was not placed, in the target-absent trials). After the target was ready, the participant was prompted by the vibration motor signal and allowed to produce clicks for 10 seconds by pressing the “5” key on the numeric keypad with their dominant hand. Another vibration signaled the end of the 10 seconds of the trial, and the participant pressed the “6” key on the keypad for presence or the “4” key for absence. The responses and number of clicks were recorded by the computer. We calculated the dis- criminability index d (Green & Swets, 1974) as the difference between the z-transforms of the hit and false alarm rate; d ¼ z (hit rate) – z (false alarm rate). The hit and false alarm rates were converted from 1 to 1 – 1/(2N) and from 0 to 1/(2N)(Macmillan & Creelman, 1991). Participants received feedback by vibration upon providing a correct answer for target presence/absence during the practice trials to familiarize themselves with detection task pro- cedure. However, they received no such feedback during the experimental trials. Visual spatial working memory task. The visual working memory capacity task was conducted on the first or third day of the echolocation task depending on each participant’s availability. Each participant performed a total of 216 trials consisting of 54 trials per condition (three, four, five, or six of the memory set size). As shown in Figure 2, each trial began with the presentation of a central fixation cross (0.2  0.2 ) for 1,000 ms, followed by the sample stimulus array for 200 ms. After a blank period of 1,000 ms, the test cue remained on the screen until the participant responded. The participants indicated whether the cued color was 8 i-Perception 10(4) on the right or left by pressing the keys for “i” (for right) or “e” (for left) responses. We computed the estimating capacity score of visual working memory with the common formula: K ¼ S (P – 50)/50, where K is the visual capacity, S is the memory set size, and P is percentage correct (Cowan, 2001). The formula represents the individual’s ability to hold K items from a sample array of S items in their working memory. Results Detection Performance Across 2 Days The d values obtained from individual participants (n ¼ 36) were averaged separately for each target distance. Figure 4(a) shows the detection performance of the echolocation task on the first and second days. To test the improvement in detection performance across the 2 days, we subjected the d scores to a 2 (Day 1 and 2)  4 (Target distance 20, 30, 40, and (a) (b) (d) (c) Figure 4. (a) Mean d score for Day 1 or 2 as a function of target distance (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm). Error bars represent SEMs. (b) Box-and-whisker plots showing the median, interquartile, and range with 1.5  interquartile range whiskers for the d score averaged across the 2 test days. (c) Mean number of clicks for Day 1 or 2 as a function of target distance (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm). Error bars represent SEMs. (d) Plots of 0 0 the increase in d score averaged across the 2 test days as a function of d on Day 1. The solid line represents the regression line of the plot. Maezawa and Kawahara 9 50 cm) repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). The ANOVA revealed significant main effect of day, F(1, 35) ¼ 9.22, p ¼ .005, g ¼ .21, and target distance, F(3, 105) ¼ 17.27, 2 2 p< .001, g ¼ .33. Their interaction was not significant, F(3, 105) ¼ 0.80, p ¼ .494, g ¼.02. p p Multiple comparisons by Holm’s method revealed that the d score decreased from 20 to 30 cm, t(35) ¼ 3.92, p ¼ .002, r ¼ .55; from 20 to 40 cm, t(35) ¼ 6.54, p< .001, r ¼.74; and from 20 to 50 cm, t(35) ¼ 5.49, p< .001, r ¼ .68. The d score averaged across both days fluctuated substantially within participants (box- plot, Figure 4(b)); the means (SD) were 1.82 (0.87), 1.44 (0.85), 1.20 (0.84), and 1.20 (0.81) when the target was located at 20, 30, 40, and 50 cm, respectively. These scores were signif- icantly higher than 0 when the target was placed at 20 cm, t(35) ¼ 12.59, p< .001, r ¼ .91; at 30 cm, t(35) ¼ 10.11, p< .001, r ¼ .86; at 40 cm, t(35) ¼ 8.62, p< .001, r ¼ .82; and at 50 cm, t(35) ¼ 8.87, p< .001, r ¼ .83. We also investigated the ceiling effect for improvement in detection performance. Participants who performed well on Day 1 improved less than those whose initial perfor- mance was poorer. The increase in detection performance was calculated by subtracting the 0 0 0 d of Day 1 from the d of Day 2. Figure 4(c) shows a plot of the increase in d score as a function of d on Day 1. We computed Pearson’s correlation coefficients, and the results showed a significant negative correlation between the observed increase in performance and d score on Day 1, r(36)¼.50, p ¼ .002, indicating that high-performing individuals improved their performance less across the 2 days than did low-performance individuals. We also assessed the effects of increasing in the number of clicks during the detection task. The participants’ number of emission sounds per trial are shown in Figure 4(d), as a function of target distances during the target trials. The mean number of clicks per trial was subjected to a repeated-measures ANOVA with a 2 (Day 1 and 2)  4 (Target distance 20, 30, 40, and 50 cm) design. The results revealed a significant effect of target distance, F(3, 105) ¼ 2.96, 2 2 p ¼ .036, g ¼.08; while a no significant effect of day, F(1, 35) ¼ 0.68, p ¼ .414, g ¼ .02, and p p their interaction, F(3, 105) ¼ 1.54, p ¼.210, g ¼.04. There was no significant correlation between the mean number of clicks per trial averaged across the distances and the d score, r(36) ¼.09, p ¼ .607; this indicated that an increase in the number of clicks did not improve or impair detection performance. Detection Performance on the Third Day The d on Day 3 obtained from individual participants were averaged separately for each target distance. Figure 5 shows the detection performance of the echolocation task on Days 2 and 3 in the 24 samples who completed the follow-up echolocation task on the third day. To examine maintaining improved detection performance over a month, we subjected the d scores to a repeated-measures ANOVA with a 3 (Day 2 and 3)  4 (Target distance 20, 30, 40, and 50 cm) design. The ANOVA revealed significant main effect of target distance, F(3, 69) ¼ 16.34, p< .001, g ¼ .42; while no significant effect of day, F(1, 23) ¼ 0.56, 2 2 p ¼ .461, g ¼ .02, and their interaction, F(6, 69) ¼ 1.50, p ¼ .222, g ¼.06, which indicated p p that improved detection performance was retained for at least 1 month. Visual Working Memory Capacity We examined the association between visual working memory capacity and object detection performance by echolocation, separately for each experimental day (Days 1–3). We also examined whether improved detection performance across 2 days was modulated by working memory capacity. Plots of detection performance scores as a function of the capacity K are 10 i-Perception 10(4) Figure 5. Mean d score of the 24 samples for Day 2 or 3 as a function of target distance (20, 30, 40, or 50 cm). Error bars represent SEMs. shown in Figure 6. The mean performance (K) on the task was 2.38 (95% CI [2.10, 2.65], SD ¼ 0.65). The results revealed a significant positive correlation between the visual working memory capacity score and the d score on Day 1, r(24) ¼ .48, p ¼ .017; and on Day 3, r(23) ¼ .49, p ¼ .018; there was no significant correlation between visual working memory capacity score and either the detection performance on Day 2, r(24) ¼ .30, p ¼ .155, or the increase in performance, r(24)¼.20, p ¼ .354. Supplementary Experiment To examine whether visual working memory capacity modulated detection performance on echolocation tasks, we further measured each individual’s visual cognitive performance using a matrix test (Cornoldi, Cortesi, & Preti, 1991), the score of which can be taken as a measure of spatial working memory capacity. In this test, participants were asked to follow an imaginary pathway through two-dimensional matrices. Twelve of the 36 original participants performed the matrix test. In addition, five students who did not perform the initial echolocation task participated in the same echolocation task for only 1 day as well as in the matrix test. Total of 17 participants (18–26 years; 7 females) completed the matrix test on the second day of the experiment or the earlier, depending on each participant’s availability. We examined the cor- relation between the performances on the matrix test and on echolocation task on the first day. Figure 7 shows a schematic diagram of a trial of the matrix test. All stimuli were displayed on a black background on an LCD monitor (XL2411T; BenQ; 100 Hz refresh rate, 1,920 1,080 pixels). Participants were required to memorize a target location displayed in acell of an 11  11 matrix, subtending 24  24 in width and height of the visual angle. Their task was to mentally maneuver the target according to visual directional cues. Each trial began with the presentation of a central fixation cross (0.2  0.2 ) for 1,000 ms, followed by the target stimulus in the matrix for 5,000 ms. The target was a red square, the initial location of which was randomly selected from the matrix. After a blank screen was shown for 1,000 ms, arrow cues (9  9 ) were sequentially presented in the center of the screen. The set size of the series of the cues (4, 6, or 8) and direction (right, left, up, or down) were assigned Maezawa and Kawahara 11 (a) (b) (c) (d) (e) Figure 6. Behavioral results of the visual working memory task. (a) Mean visual working memory capacity (K) as a function of memory set size (3, 4, 5, or 6). Error bars represent SEMs. (b–d) Correlations between visual working memory capacity and d for Day 1, Day 2, or Day 3. It should be noted that several points overlapped each other. The solid line represents the regression line of the plot. (e) Correlation between visual working memory and increase in d across the 2 test days. randomly. Each directional cue was presented for 1,000 ms, and the cues were separated by a blank screen presented for 1,000 ms. After the presentation of a series of directional cues, the matrix was displayed on its own. Participants indicated the final target destination by clicking a cell in the matrix. Each participant performed a total of 30 trials consisting of 10 trials of each set size of the sequence length. The proportions of correct responses obtained from individual participants were averaged across the sequence length. The mean correct proportion was 0.82 (95% CI [0.76, 0.88], SD ¼ 0.11). One participant was removed from the analysis as an outlier, indicated by a Cook’s distance exceeding 0.5. We examined the association between the mean proportion of correct responses and echolocation performance on the first day (n¼ 16). The plot of the d scores on Day 1 against the mean proportion of correct responses is shown in Figure 8. We computed Pearson’s correlation coefficients, and the results revealed a significant positive correlation between the matrix test score and the d score on Day 1, r(16)¼ .50, p¼ .049. Discussion We examined the association between echolocation performance in sighted individuals and individual factors, such as visual working memory capacity. We found that the participants’ visual working memory capacity was positively correlated with detection performance, 12 i-Perception 10(4) Direconal cues …. 1000 ms 1000 ms 1000 ms 1000 ms Fixaon cross Target Blank Test Direconal cues 1000 ms 5000 ms 1000 ms Unl Response Time Figure 7. Schematic diagram of the matrix test. Participants followed an imaginary pathway through two- dimensional matrices (11  11; the example shows a scaled down 5  5 grid). The participants were required to identify the final destination of the target. (a) (b) Figure 8. Behavioral results of the matrix test. (a) Mean proportion of correct responses as a function of memory set size (4, 6, or 8). Error bars represent SEMs. (b) Correlation between the proportion of correct response and echolocation performance (d ) on Day 1. The solid line represents the regression line of the plot. Maezawa and Kawahara 13 expect on the second day. Detection performance did not decline over several months. In other words, we found that the improved detection achieved on the second day was maintained for at least 30 to 137 days. Interestingly, detection performance improved over the localization task repeated for 120 trials (approximately 50 minutes) per day without any feedback, although participants did not perform as well as subjects in a previous study that included feedback on every trial (Schenkman & Nilsson, 2010). This indicates that sighted participants can improve their performance following a relatively small amount of repetitive training during the task. However, we found individual differences in detection performance. Specifically, individuals’ performances on target detection tests fluctuated substantially, reflected by the large stan- dard deviations and range values of the data. The present results extend the findings of Ekkel et al. (2017), in that we assessed spatial working memory capacity; participants required to memorize visual objects being actively used during visual spatial processing while not involving auditory verbal processing. Our results show a significant correlation between visual working memory capacity and detection performance during echolocation, except for performance on the second day, after performance had improved. The correlation between visual working memory capacity and performance on the second day was not significant, and the coefficient value decreased compared to the first day. This lack of a correlation on the second day might be due to the improvement in performance. Individuals who performed well on the first day of the tests improved less than those who initially had low scores, and thus the regression line of detec- tion performance on working memory capacity flattened out on the second day, decreasing the correlation coefficient value. Our results were inconsistent with those of by Ekkel et al. (2017), in that we did not find a significant correlation between working memory capacity and the increase in detection per- formance across the 2 days. This difference seems reasonable because of the ceiling effect for enhancement of echolocation ability. Specifically, because there was a positive correlation between echolocation performance and working memory capacity on the first day, individ- uals who had large memory capacities were expected to show a smaller increase in detection performance across the 2 days. In fact, the results indicated a negative (but nonsignificant) correlation (r¼.20). Another possible explanation is that the study by Ekkel et al. (2017) assessed participants’ working memory capacity in the context of the central executive com- ponent, rather than a spatial subcomponent, whereas this study measured the working memory capacity as a reflection of spatial aspects. Also, Ekkel et al. assessed performance using a size discrimination task, whereas this study used a different type of task, involving object detection (Schenkman & Nilsson, 2010). Additional research is needed to determine the influence of types of echolocation and working memory tasks on correlations between working memory capacity and echolocation ability. In summary, visual working memory capacity predicts object detection performance on an echolocation task. The present finding adds to the literature in that the correlation between echolocation ability, rather than the degree of improvement in echolocation due to training, and visual working memory capacity has never been examined. The involvement of vision during echolocation processing has been suggested by Thaler et al. (2014), who indicated that echolocation involves spatial cognitive processing (e.g., mental imagery). The visual area may not be involved during other auditory control tasks (e.g., change in the sound location; Thaler & Foresteire, 2017). The present results support these findings regarding visual involvement in echolocation and the spatial component of working memory by sighted participants. Furthermore, the findings suggest that visual working memory capacity is predictive of higher performance of the detection task for sighted participants. 14 i-Perception 10(4) The question of how much training is necessary to learn echolocation remains unresolved. To clarify this issue, future studies should manipulate the amount of echolocation task training to determine a sufficient amount of training as well as to investigate how perfor- mance improves. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This work was supported by Graduate Grant Program of Graduate School of Letters, Hokkaido University, Japan (to T. M.) and by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (17H02648) (to J. I. K.). ORCID iD Tomoki Maezawa https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9077-3132 References Au, W. W. L., Branstetter, B. K., Benoit-Bird, K. J., & Kastelein, R. A. (2009). Acoustic basis for fish prey discrimination by echolocating dolphins and porpoises. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 126, 460–467. doi:10.1121/1.3147497 Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422 Barclay, R. M. R., & Brigham, R. M. (1994). Constraints on optimal foraging: Am, field test of prey discrimination by echolocating insectivorous bats. Animal Behaviour, 48, 1013–1021. doi:10.1006/ anbe.1994.1334 Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436. doi:10.1163/ 156856897X00357 Carlson-Smith, C., & Weiner, W. R. (1996). The auditory skills necessary for echolocation: A new explanation. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 90, 21–35. Cornoldi, C., Cortesi, A., & Preti, D. (1991). Individual differences in the capacity limitations of visuospatial short-term memory: Research on sighted and totally congenitally blind people. Memory & Cognition, 19, 459–468. doi:10.3758/BF03199569 Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003922 Ekkel, M. R., van Lier, R., & Steenbergen, B. (2017). Learning to echolocate in sighted people: A correlational study on attention, working memory and spatial abilities. Experimental Brain Research, 235, 809–818. doi:10.1007/s00221-016-4833-z Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1974). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Oxford, England: Robert E. Krieger. Gronwall, D. M. (1977). Paced auditory serial-addition task: A measure of recovery from concussion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 367–373. doi:10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367 Kolarik, A. J., Cirstea, S., Pardhan, S., & Moore, B. C. J. (2014). A summary of research investigating echolocation abilities of blind and sighted humans. Hearing Research, 310, 60–68. doi:10.1016/j. heares.2014.01.010 Maezawa and Kawahara 15 Kolarik, A. J., Moore, B. C. J., Zahorik, P., Cirstea, S., & Pardhan, S. (2016). Auditory distance perception in humans: A review of cues, development, neuronal bases, and effects of sensory loss. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78, 373–395. doi:0.3758/s13414-015-1015-1 Kolarik, A. J., Scarfe, A. C., Moore, B. C. J., & Pardhan, S. (2016). An assessment of auditory-guided locomotion in an obstacle circumvention task. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 1725–1735. doi:10.1007/s00221-016-4567-y Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14, 389–433. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(05)80012-1 Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., McHugh, B., & Giudice, N. A. (2012). Spatial working memory for locations specified by vision and audition: Testing the amodality hypothesis. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1260–1267. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0311-2 Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunc- tions. Nature, 390, 279–281. doi:10.1038/36846 Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (2013). Visual working memory capacity: From psychophysics and neuro- biology to individual differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 391–400. doi:10.1016/j. tics.2013.06.006 Luria, R., & Vogel, E. K. (2011). Visual search demands dictate reliance on working memory storage. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 6199–6207. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6453-10.2011 Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Mance, I., & Vogel, E. K. (2013). Visual working memory. WIREs Cognitive Science, 4, 179–190. doi:10.1002/wcs.1219 Moossavi, A., Mehrkian, S., Lotfi, Y., Faghihzadeh, S., & Sajedi, H. (2014). The relation between working memory capacity and auditory lateralization in children with auditory processing disorders. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 78, 1981–1986. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2014.09.003 Neuweiler, G. (1990). Auditory adaptations for prey capture in echolocating bats. Physiological Reviews, 70, 615–641. doi:10.1152/physrev.1990.70.3.615 Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442. doi:10.1163/156856897X00366 Rice, C. E. (1967). Human echo perception. Science, 155, 656–664. doi:10.1126/science.155.3763.656 Rice, C. E., & Feinstein, S. H. (1965). Sonar system of the blind: Size discrimination. Science, 148, 1107–1108. doi:10.1126/science.148.3673.1107 Ricker, T. J., AuBuchon, A. M., & Cowan, N. (2010). Working memory. WIREs Cognitive Science, 1, 573–585. doi:10.1002/wcs.50 Rojas, J. A. M., Hermosilla, J. A., Montero, R. S., & Espı, P. L. L. (2009). Physical analysis of several organic signals for human echolocation: Oral vacuum pulses. Acta Acustica United With Acustica, 95, 325–330. doi:10.3813/AAA.918155 Schenkman, B. N., & Nilsson, M. E. (2010). Human echolocation: Blind and sighted persons’ ability to detect sounds recorded in the presence of a reflecting object. Perception, 39, 483–501. doi:10.1068/p6473 Serino, A., Ciaramelli, E., Santantonio, A. D., Malagu,  S., Servadei, F., & La` davas, E. (2007). A pilot study for rehabilitation of central executive deficits after traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 21, 11–19. doi:10.1080/02699050601151811 Tao, Q., Chan, C. C. H., Luo, Y.-j., Li, J.-j., Ting, K.-h., Wang, J., & Lee, T. M. C. (2015). How does experience modulate auditory spatial processing in individuals with blindness? Brain Topography, 28, 506–519. doi:10.1007/s10548-013-0339-1 Teng, S., Puri, A., & Whitney, D. (2012). Ultrafine spatial acuity of blind expert human echolocators. Experimental Brain Research, 216, 483–488. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2951-1 Teng, S., & Whitney, D. (2011). The acuity of echolocation: Spatial resolution in the sighted compared to expert performance. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 105, 20–32. doi:10.1177/ 0145482X1110500103 16 i-Perception 10(4) Thaler, L., & Castillo-Serrano, J. (2016). People’s ability to detect objects using click-based echoloca- tion: A direct comparison between mouth-clicks and clicks made by a loudspeaker. PLoS One, 11, e0154868. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0154868 Thaler, L., & Foresteire, D. (2017). Visual sensory stimulation interferes with people’s ability to echolocate object size. Scientific Reports, 7, 13069. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-12967-3 Thaler, L., Wilson, R. C., & Gee, B. K. (2014). Correlation between vividness of visual imagery and echolocation ability in sighted, echo-naive people. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 1915–1925. doi:10.1007/s00221-014-3883-3 Tonelli, A., Brayda, L., & Gori, M. (2016). Depth echolocation learnt by novice sighted people. PLoS One, 11, e0156654. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0156654 Tonelli, A., Campus, C., & Brayda, L. (2018). How body motion influences echolocation while walking. Scientific Reports, 8, 15704. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-34074-7 Tsubomi, H., Fukuda, K., Watanabe, K., & Vogel, E. K. (2013). Neural limits to representing objects still within view. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 8257–8263. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5348-12.2013 Unsworth, N., Fukuda, K., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2014). Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval. Cognitive Psychology, 71, 1–26. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.01.003 Vogel, E. K., McCullough, A. W., & Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature, 438, 500–503. doi:10.1038/nature04171 Worchel, P., Mauney, J., & Andrew, J. G. (1950). The perception of obstacles by the blind. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 746–751. doi:10.1037/h0060950 Zahorik, P., Bangayan, P., Sundareswaran, V., Wang, K., & Tam, C. (2006). Perceptual recalibration in human sound localization: Learning to remediate front-back reversals. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 343–359. doi:10.1121/1.2208429 How to cite this article Maezawa, T., & Kawahara, J. I. (2019). Effects of visual working memory on individual differences in echolocation performance in sighted participants. i-Perception, 10(4), 1–16. doi: 10.1177/

Journal

i-PerceptionSAGE

Published: Aug 29, 2019

Keywords: echolocation; perception; visual spatial working memory

There are no references for this article.