Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

The Relationships between Cognitive Styles and Creativity: The Role of Field Dependence-Independence on Visual Creative Production

The Relationships between Cognitive Styles and Creativity: The Role of Field... behavioral sciences Article The Relationships between Cognitive Styles and Creativity: The Role of Field Dependence-Independence on Visual Creative Production 1 , 1 2 , 3 1 Marco Giancola * , Massimiliano Palmiero , Laura Piccardi and Simonetta D’Amico Department of Biotechnological and Applied Clinical Sciences, University of L’Aquila, 67100 L’Aquila, Italy; massimiliano.palmiero@univaq.it (M.P.); simonetta.damico@univaq.it (S.D.) Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, 00185 Rome, Italy; laura.piccardi@uniroma1.it Cognitive and Motor Rehabilitation and Neuroimaging Unit, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, 00179 Rome, Italy * Correspondence: marco.giancola@graduate.univaq.it Abstract: Previous studies explored the relationships between field dependent-independent cognitive style (FDI) and creativity, providing misleading and unclear results. The present research explored this problematic interplay through the lens of the Geneplore model, employing a product-oriented task: the Visual Creative Synthesis Task (VCST). The latter requires creating objects belonging to pre-established categories, starting from triads of visual components and consists of two steps: the preinventive phase and the inventive phase. Following the Amabile’s consensual assessment technique, three independent judges evaluated preinventive structures in terms of originality and synthesis whereas inventions were evaluated in terms of originality and appropriateness. The Em- bedded Figure Test (EFT) was employed in order to measure the individual’s predisposition toward the field dependence or the field independence. Sixty undergraduate college students (31 females) took part in the experiment. Results revealed that field independent individuals outperformed field dependent ones in each of the four VCST scores, showing higher levels of creativity. Results were Citation: Giancola, M.; Palmiero, M.; discussed in light of the better predisposition of field independent individuals in mental imagery, Piccardi, L.; D’Amico, S. The mental manipulation of abstract objects, as well as in using their knowledge during complex tasks Relationships between Cognitive that require creativity. Future research directions were also discussed. Styles and Creativity: The Role of Field Dependence-Independence on Keywords: field dependent-independent cognitive styles; creativity; creative production; Geneplore Visual Creative Production. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212. https://doi.org/ model; visual creative synthesis task 10.3390/bs12070212 Academic Editor: Scott D. Lane Received: 18 April 2022 1. Introduction Accepted: 22 June 2022 Creativity has been widely recognized as the key to success in contemporary society, Published: 25 June 2022 affecting art, science, economy, and everyday problem solving [1,2]. Given its relevance Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral in human activities, creativity has received growing attention since the second half of the with regard to jurisdictional claims in 20th century, when Guilford proposed the multifactorial Structure of Intellect Model [3], in published maps and institutional affil- which creative thinking encompassed convergent thinking (CT) and divergent thinking iations. (DT). Whereas CT is stated as the ability to converge on prevailing ways of thinking in order to find a single, right, and ready-made solution to a problem that other people could also reach, DT represents a spontaneous and free-flowing form of thought and exemplifies the ability to find many new solutions to an open-ended problem. Additionally, DT is Copyright: © 2022 by the authors. widely recognized as one of the main indicators of people’s creative potential [4], informing Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. about the likelihood that one can act “outside the box”. Although Guilford’s work has This article is an open access article represented a milestone in the literature of creativity, researchers have suggested alternative distributed under the terms and frameworks [5,6], including the Geneplore model [7]. The latter represents one of the most conditions of the Creative Commons influential developments in the tradition of cognitive psychology [8], and exemplifies Attribution (CC BY) license (https:// a twofold framework in which real-world creative production involves a cyclic motion creativecommons.org/licenses/by/ between generative and explorative phases. The generative phase plays a crucial role in 4.0/). Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12070212 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 2 of 12 producing pre-inventive structures that are internal prototypes of inventions characterized by different degrees of creative potential and originality [9]. Whereas this phase requires several cognitive resources, including memory retrieval, mental synthesis, mental trans- formation, and categorical reduction [10], the explorative phase, employing cognitive and meta-cognitive processes such as attribute finding, conceptual interpretation, functional inference, and hypothesis testing, drives the examination and practical interpretation of preinventive structures to generate a creative outcome [11]. According to the product perspective [12,13], two main criteria are necessary to evaluate creative inventions: origi- nality and appropriateness. Whereas the former encompasses the degree of novelty and uncommonness of productions, the latter refers to the relevance and usefulness, exemplify- ing the individual ability to produce an outcome that fits the needs and constraints of a given situation and is well situated in a given context [14]. Notably, even though different attributes for evaluating creative inventions can be found in the literature of creative pro- duction (e.g., elegance, aesthetics, and surprise), originality and appropriateness are the most accurate criteria, which reflect the full standard definition of creativity [15]. Given the multifaceted nature of creativity, the impact of cognitive factors on creative accomplishment has been long discussed in the past [16]. Cognitive style, also known as thinking style, represents a pivotal factor unquestionably related to creativity [17]. The expression cognitive style refers to how people acquire, organize, and use information [18]. Cognitive styles are usually conceptualized as bipolar, pervasive, and relatively stable over time, representing a critical dimension of the individual functioning. Although the key role of cognitive styles in human cognition and behavior has been extensively acknowledged, their universal explanatory power has not consistently been demonstrated. However, amongst all cognitive styles, the universal explanatory power of the field dependent- independent cognitive style (FDI) has been widely recognized [19]. In their seminal work, Witkin and colleagues defined FDI as “the extent to which the person perceives part of a field as discrete from the surrounding field as a whole, rather than embedded in the field” [20] (pp. 6–7). This cognitive style describes a stable and habitual tendency [21,22] characterized by two different poles: field dependence and field independence. Unlike field dependent subjects (FDs), field independent individuals (FIs) usually show less diffi- culty in separating information from the surrounding context [23] and are generally more focused on relevant information, inhibiting attention to irrelevant information coming from the environment [24]. Although FIs are generally defined as more flexible, open- minded, and capable of breaking down the routine than FDs, empirical evidence on the role of FDI on creativity provided unclear results [21], probably because of the involvement of specific cognitive processes (e.g., fluid intelligence, inhibition, working memory, and flexibility [25,26]), socio-cultural factors (e.g., Western vs. Eastern [27]), as well as the discrepancy between the scoring methods (e.g., empirically-based and rater-based scoring methods [28]), tasks used (e.g., divergent and convergent tasks, real-world creative produc- tion tasks [29]), and sampling bias. For a systematic review on the role of FDI in creativity, see Giancola et al. [30]. Regarding DT, some studies revealed that FIs outperformed FDs [31,32] in gener- ating ideas, whereas others found non-significant results [33,34]. For instance, Li and colleagues [19] revealed that FIs outperformed FDs in scientific and social brainstorming tasks in terms of fluency and novelty, confirming previous research [32,35]. In addition, Lei and colleagues [17] found that field independence was related to fluency and originality but not to flexibility, whereas Niaz and colleagues [34] found no significant effect of FDI. Regarding CT, some authors stressed that FIs attained significantly higher scores than FDs in convergent measures [36,37], others found no significant effect of FDI [38]. Finally, for real-world creative production, to the knowledge of the current research, only two studies explored the impact of FDI on creative production. Specifically, Miller ’s study [39] showed that FIs reported higher creativity scores than FDs in the creative collage making task. Similarly, Giancola et al. [29] found that FIs outperformed FDs in the ability to generate real-world creative objects. Notably, even though some authors found positive correlations Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 3 of 12 amongst FDI and self-rated artistic abilities and artistic competencies [40,41], research on the impact of FDI on creative production remains scattered to date. Therefore, the present research aimed to shed further light on the issue, employing the logic of the Geneplore model, which involves the combined effect of generation and exploration of ideas to generate real-world visual creative objects. Compared to Giancola et al. [29], who used a one-step procedure, priming participants with object category names while combining the visual stimuli, in the present study a two-step procedure was used. Specifically, participants were firstly instructed to construct pre-inventive forms combining the visual stimuli, and then interpret them within a specific conceptual category. This led to a better understanding of the role of FDI in the creative process, which encompasses both a visuo-spatial generative phase of undefined ideas and a conceptual/inferential phase of refined ideas. In this vein, the generative phase requires mainly divergent thinking to generate preinventive structures, whereas the explorative phase requires mainly convergent thinking to define and evaluate actual inventions [42]. Based on previous studies, the first two hypotheses were formulated as follows: Hypotheses 1 (H1). FIs outperform FDs in the preinventive phase because FIs are more divergent, thus more able in generating ideas than FDs [31,32]; Hypotheses 2 (H2). FIs outperform FDs in the inventive phase because FIs are also more conver- gent, thus more analytic in evaluating ideas than FDs [36,37]; Hypotheses 3 (H3). FIs do not outperform FDs, regardless of the phase of the creative process, because previous studies found inconsistent relationships between FDI and both divergent and convergent measures of the creative process [33,34,38]. 2. Materials and Methods 2.1. Participants and Procedure Sixty undergraduate college students attending different courses at The University of L’Aquila (L’Aquila, Italy) participated in the study (mean age = 22.30  3.31; age range = 19–32). Twenty-nine of them were males (48.3%), and thirty-one were females (51.7%). After signing the written informed consent to participate in the study, all participants were asked to complete an anamnesis questionnaire assessing biographical and educational information, general health state, and background or formal achievement in art. No participant reported psychiatric or neurological disorders, drug or alcohol addictions, and no participants declared a background or formal achievement in art. The experimental protocol was administered individually to each participant in a quiet room of the Socio- Cognitive Processes in Life Span Laboratory at The University of L’Aquila (L’Aquila, Italy). The experiment lasted approximately 45 min. The Local Ethics Committee approved this experiment in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. 2.2. Measures 2.2.1. Assessment of Field Dependent Independent Cognitive Style The Embedded Figure Test—EFT [43] is usually employed to evaluate the individual’s predisposition toward the field dependent or the field independent cognitive style. It is a paper and pencil test in which participants were requested to find a simple black and white shape within a geometric colored complex figure. The test consists of 24 cards (12 cards with simple shapes and 12 cards with complex figures) 12.9  7.7 cm (see Figure 1). Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 4 of 12 4 of 12 Figure 1. An example of an item taken from the Embedded Figure Test (EFT). (A) The geometric Figure 1. An example of an item taken from the Embedded Figure Test (EFT). (A) The geometric colored complex figure. (B) The simple black and white simple shape. (C) The simple shape within colored complex figure. (B) The simple black and white simple shape. (C) The simple shape within the complex figure. the complex figure. The experimenter presented the complex-colored figure one by one for 15 s and the The experimenter presented the complex-colored figure one by one for 15 s and the participant had to describe the figure in a loud voice. Then, the experimenter removed the participant had to describe the figure in a loud voice. Then, the experimenter removed the complex figure and presented the simple one; after 10 s, he took away the simple black and complex figure and presented the simple one; after 10 s, he took away the simple black white shape and presented once again the complex-colored figure. After that, participants and white shape and presented once again the complex-colored figure. After that, partic- had to find the simple black and white shape embedded in the complex figure. They were ipants had to find the simple black and white shape embedded in the complex figure. instructed to inform the experimenter as soon as they found the figure and trace its outlines They were instructed to inform the experimenter as soon as they found the figure and using a pencil. When the participants declared to have found the simple black and white trace its outlines using a pencil. When the participants declared to have found the simple shape within the complex figure, the experimenter annotated the elapsed time (timing). If black and white shape within the complex figure, the experimenter annotated the elapsed the response (tracing of the outlines) was wrong, the experimenter continued to take the time until the participant provided the correct response or until 180 s had elapsed. The total time (timing). If the response (tracing of the outlines) was wrong, the experimenter con- time was divided by the number of items (12) in order to compute the average time (RTs) tinued to take the time until the participant provided the correct response or until 180 s which was used as the measure of the individual’s cognitive style. A shorter time indicated had elapsed. The total time was divided by the number of items (12) in order to compute a higher predisposition towards field independence, whereas a longer time indicated a the average time (RTs) which was used as the measure of the individual’s cognitive style. higher predisposition towards field dependence. A shorter time indicated a higher predisposition towards field independence, whereas a longer time indicated a higher predisposition towards field dependence. 2.2.2. Assessment of Creativity The Visual Creative Synthesis Task—VCST [7] aimed to create objects belonging to 2.2.2. Assessment of Creativity pre-established categories, starting from triads of visual components. The task consists of two steps: the preinventive phase and the inventive phase. Following Palmiero and The Visual Creative Synthesis Task—VCST [7] aimed to create objects belonging to colleagues [44], three triads of components and three categories were used. Preinventive pre-established categories, starting from triads of visual components. The task consists of phase: participants, after a practical example, were asked to combine the components into a two steps: the preinventive phase and the inventive phase. Following Palmiero and col- preinventive structure, one for each triad. They could be changed in position, rotation, and leagues [44], three triads of components and three categories were used. Preinventive size but not in their general structure. The triads of components were presented along with phase: participants, after a practical example, were asked to combine the components into their name on a paper sheet (see Figure 2). Participants had 15 s to fix and memorize the a preinventive structure, one for each triad. They could be changed in position, rotation, components and 2 min to think of the preinventive structure for each triad. Inventive phase: and size but not after in thcr eieating r genethe ral thr stree ucpr tur einventive e. The tristr aductur s of c es, om participants ponents wwer ere eppr reesented sented with along a category name for each of them (1 furniture, 1 weapon, and 1 sport goods) and were requested to with their name on a paper sheet (see Figure 2). Participants had 15 s to fix and memorize think of their inventions. Participants had 3 min to describe the functioning of inventions the components and 2 min to think of the preinventive structure for each triad. Inventive (1 min for each invention), also providing their title (see Figure 3). The description of the phase: after creating the three preinventive structures, participants were presented with a objects was also defined in terms of length of responses (i.e., number of words). The order category name for each of them (1 furniture, 1 weapon, and 1 sport goods) and were re- of the triads was randomized. quested to think of their inventions. Participants had 3 min to describe the functioning of inventions (1 min for each invention), also providing their title (see Figure 3). The descrip- tion of the objects was also defined in terms of length of responses (i.e., number of words). The order of the triads was randomized. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 5 of 12 Beh Behav av. .SSci. ci. 2 2022 022,, 112 2,, 2 212 12 5 of 12 5 of 12 Figure 2. The three triads of components for the Visual Creative Synthesis Task (VCST): (1) cube, Figure 2. The three triads of components for the Visual Creative Synthesis Task (VCST): (1) cube, braF cig ke u tr , ec on 2. Th e (e s p tor hree t g t ood riad ss ); of (2 c ) om pap ra on llel en ep ts ip for ed t,h d e y V -p isy ur aa l m Cird e,a th iv or e n S y (fu nth rn es itiu s rTa e); s k (3 (V ) s C tr SiT): p, t(r 1a ) p cez ub oi e,d , cyl bracket, b in rd ac er ke (w t,cone c eon ape o (sport n (ss p ). or goods); t goods); (2 ) (2 parallelepiped, ) parallelepiped dy-pyramid, , dy-pyrami horn d, hor (furnitur n (furne); itur (3 e); ) strip, (3) sttrapezoid, rip, trapezoid, cylinder (weapons). cylinder (weapons). Figure 3. An example of creative production based on the triad n.3 made up of one stripe, one Figure 3. An example of creative production based on the triad n.3 made up of one stripe, one trap- trapezoid, and one cylinder. Category: Weapons; Title: Grenade; Description: This cylinder is a Figure 3. An example of creative production based on the triad n.3 made up of one stripe, one trap- ezoid, and one cylinder. Category: Weapons; Title: Grenade; Description: This cylinder is a grenade. grenade. The cylinder contains the explosive, the trapezoid is the trigger mechanism, and the strip ezoid, and one cylinder. Category: Weapons; Title: Grenade; Description: This cylinder is a grenade. The cylinder contains the explosive, the trapezoid is the trigger mechanism, and the strip controls contr The ols cylithe nder whole contgr ain enade s the and explhas osiv ae, safety the tr function. apezoid is the trigger mechanism, and the strip controls the whole grenade and has a safety function. the whole grenade and has a safety function. Based on Amabile’s consensual assessment technique [45], three independent judges, two Ba females sed on and Ama one bile male ’s co(mean nsensua age l a= ss25.33 essm en4.50), t techevaluated nique [45]pr , th einventive ree indep str en uctur dent es judges, Based on Amabile’s consensual assessment technique [45], three independent judges, and inventions. The judges were three psychology students who attended training on two females and one male (mean age = 25.33 ± 4.50), evaluated preinventive structures two females and one male (mean age = 25.33 ± 4.50), evaluated preinventive structures creativity and its assessment for a total of 20 h. During the training sessions, the main and inventions. The judges were three psychology students who attended training on cre- and inventions. The judges were three psychology students who attended training on cre- models and descriptive frameworks of creativity were explained, including the SOI and ativ ati ity vi ty an a dn id ts i ta ss a ss es se ss m sm en et nft o fr o r a a to to ta ta l lo o f f2 2 00 h h . .D Dur uriin ng g tth he e t tr ra ai in niin ng g s se essssiio on n ss , ,th th ee m m aa in in m m od od ele sl s the Geneplore Model. In addition, students were shown examples of creative productions and an d d ed sc er sic p rti ip v ti ev e fr a fr m am ew ew or ok rs k so o f fc r ce re aa titi vv it it y y w we erre e e ex xp plla ai in ne ed d,, iin nc cllud udiin n g g tt hh ee SS O O I I an an dd th th e e already evaluated in the past by judges, and they were trained to evaluate creative pro- Geneplore Model. In addition, students were shown examples of creative productions al- Geneplore Model. In addition, students were shown examples of creative productions al- ductions in terms of creativity. After the training, the evaluation sessions began. Notably, ready evaluated in the past by judges, and they were trained to evaluate creative produc- ready evaluated in the past by judges, and they were trained to evaluate creative produc- three basic parameters were used: originality and appropriateness, which are the most tions in terms of creativity. After the training, the evaluation sessions began. Notably, tions in terms of creativity. After the training, the evaluation sessions began. Notably, accurate and used criteria, reflecting the full standard definition of creativity [15], as well three basic parameters were used: originality and appropriateness, which are the most thr as ee synthesis, basic pawhich ramete was rs w used ere specifically used: origto inevaluate ality and the ap participants’ propriatene ability ss, wto hic holistically h are the most accurate and used criteria, reflecting the full standard definition of creativity [15], as well associate the elements during the preinventive phase of the VCST. Therefore, the preinven- accurate and used criteria, reflecting the full standard definition of creativity [15], as well as synthesis, which was used specifically to evaluate the participants’ ability to holistically tive structures were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms of as synthesis, which was used specifically to evaluate the participants’ ability to holistically originality associate ,th de efined elem as en ats form durbeing ing thnew e prand einvnot enti derived ve phas fr eom of something the VCST.else The (fr reom fore 1, = th very e prein- associate the elements during the preinventive phase of the VCST. Therefore, the prein- poor originality to 5 = very high originality) and synthesis, defined as the extent to which ventive structures were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms ventive structures were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms components were well assembled (from 1 = very poor synthesis to 5 = very high synthesis). of originality, defined as a form being new and not derived from something else (from 1 of originality, defined as a form being new and not derived from something else (from 1 The inter-rater correlation (intra-class correlation coefficient—absolute agreement) were = very poor originality to 5 = very high originality) and synthesis, defined as the extent to = very poor originality to 5 = very high originality) and synthesis, defined as the extent to significant for both originality (a = 0.92; p < 0.01) and synthesis (a = 0.93; p < 0.01). Inven- which components were well assembled (from 1 = very poor synthesis to 5 = very high which components were well assembled (from 1 = very poor synthesis to 5 = very high tions were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms of originality synthesis). The inter-rater correlation (intra-class correlation coefficient—absolute agree- sydefined nthesis)as . T ahpr e oduct inter-r being ater c new orreand latio not n (derived intra-cla fr som s co something rrelation c else oeff (fr icom ient 1— =a very bsolpoor ute agree- ment) were significant for both originality (α = 0.92; p < 0.01) and synthesis (α = 0.93; p < originality to 5 = very high originality), and appropriateness, defined as an invention with a ment) were significant for both originality (α = 0.92; p < 0.01) and synthesis (α = 0.93; p < 0.01). Inventions were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms practical instead of a hypothetical use (from 1 = very poor appropriateness to 5 = very high 0.01). Inventions were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms of originality defined as a product being new and not derived from something else (from of originality defined as a product being new and not derived from something else (from 1 = very poor originality to 5 = very high originality), and appropriateness, defined as an 1 = very poor originality to 5 = very high originality), and appropriateness, defined as an invention with a practical instead of a hypothetical use (from 1 = very poor appropriate- invention with a practical instead of a hypothetical use (from 1 = very poor appropriate- ness to 5 = very high appropriateness). The inter-rater correlation (intra-class correlation ness to 5 = very high appropriateness). The inter-rater correlation (intra-class correlation Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 6 of 12 appropriateness). The inter-rater correlation (intra-class correlation coefficient—absolute agreement) was significant for both originality (a = 0.94; p < 0.01) and appropriateness (a = 0.96; p < 0.01). 3. Results Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 24 for Windows (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). Data were tested for normality and all measures were normally distributed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov Test: Z VCST—Pre—Originality = 0.75, ns; Z VCST—Pre—Synthesis = 0.73, ns; Z VCST—Inv—Originality = 0.76, ns; Z VCST— Inv—Appropriateness = 0.31, ns; Z — = 0.84, ns) except for the EFT VCST Description length (RTs): (Kolmogorov–Smirnov Test: Z = 0.02, sig). Additionally, the analysis of the EFT (RTs) distribution of the EFT using the interquartile range method (IQR) revealed that only four FDs were ‘far out’ from the mean. This suggests that in the present sample data were mostly skewed toward field dependence (e.g., high RTs) rather than field independence (e.g., low RTs). Table 1 reported descriptive statistics divided for group (field dependence vs. field independence). Table 1. Descriptive statistics divided by FDI groups. FDs FIs N 30 30 Gender 21 F 10 F Age mean (SD) 21.61 (3.10) 22.41 (3.56) VCST—Pre—Originality mean (SD) 1.86 (0.37) 2.18 (0.41) VCST—Pre—Synthesis mean (SD) 1.76 (0.49) 2.25 (0.56) VCST—Inv—Originality mean (SD) 2.14 (0.59) 2.79 (0.76) VCST—Inv—Appropriateness mean (SD) 1.97 (0.66) 2.67 (0.81) VCST—Description length 44.33 (25.66) 59.10 (28.43) Note. FDs = Field Dependents; FIs = Field Independents; VCST = Visual Creative Synthesis Task; Pre = Preinven- tive phase; Inv = Inventive phase. Correlational analysis was computed using Spearman’s Rho (see Table 2). Results revealed that the EFT (RTs) was negatively correlated with the VCST—Preinventive phase Originality (r = 0.50, p < 0.01), VCST—Preinventive phase Synthesis (r = 0.48, p < 0.01), VCST—Inventive phase Originality (r = 0.51, p < 0.01) VCST—Inventive phase Appropri- ateness (r = 0.52, p < 0.01), and VCST—Description length (r = 0.30, p < 0.05). Notably, VCST—Description length positively correlated to VCST—Inventive phase Originality (r = 0.31, p < 0.05) VCST—Inventive phase Appropriateness (r = 0.29, p < 0.05). In addi- tion, gender also correlated negatively to EFT (RTs) (r = 0.44, p < 0.01) and positively to VCST—Preinventive phase Originality (r = 0.34, p < 0.01), VCST—Preinventive phase Synthesis (r = 0.35, p < 0.01), VCST—Inventive phase Originality (r = 0.27, p < 0.05) and VCST—Inventive phase Appropriateness (r = 0.29, p < 0.05), meaning that males showed higher field dependence and creativity scores than females. In order to obtain a more accurate picture of the relationship between EFT-RTs and the creativity scores, possible gender confounding effects were checked using the Spearman’s Rho partial correlations (see Table 3). The latter showed that controlling for gender had a little effect on the strength of the relationships between EFT-RTs and the creativity scores, and the significance level was not affected at all. Therefore, the variable ‘gender ’ was not further considered when analyzing the comparison between FDs and FIs in terms of creativity. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 7 of 12 Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and Spearman’s Rho inter-correlations. M SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 1. Gender - - 1 2. EFT (RTs) 41.63 29.25 0.44 ** 1 3. VCST—Pre—Originality 2.02 0.42 0.34 ** 0.50 ** 1 4. VCST—Pre—Synthesis 2.01 0.57 0.35 ** 0.48 ** 0.91 ** 1 5. VCST—Inv—Originality 2.47 0.74 0.27 * 0.51 ** 0.86 ** 0.84 ** 1 6. VCST—Inv—Appropriateness 2.34 0.81 0.29 * 0.52 ** 0.82 ** 0.85 ** 0.93 ** 1 7. VCST—Description length 52.02 27.79 0.02 0.30 * 0.15 0.15 0.31 * 0.29 * 1 Note. N = 60, gender was dummy coded (F = 0, M = 1) * p < 0.05 (two tailed) ** p < 0.01 (two tailed) EFT = Embedded Figure Test RTs = Response Times: VCST = Visual Creative Synthesis Task Pre = Preinventive phase Inv = Inventive phase. Table 3. Spearman’s Rho partial correlations. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 1. EFT (RTs) 2. VCST—Pre—Originality 0.41 ** 1 3. VCST—Pre—Synthesis 0.38 ** 0.90 ** 1 4. VCST—Inv—Originality 0.46 ** 0.85 ** 0.83 ** 1 5. VCST—Inv—Appropriateness 0.46 ** 0.80 ** 0.84 ** 0.93 ** 1 6. VCST—Description length 0.35 ** 0.16 0.17 0.33 * 0.31 * 1 Note. N = 60, * p < 0.05 (two tailed) ** p < 0.01 (two tailed) EFT = Embedded Figure Test RTs = Response Times: VCST = Visual Creative Synthesis Task Pre = Preinventive phase Inv = Inventive phase. Following Tascón and colleagues [46], since the EFT does not have a scale to divide FIs and FDs and taking into consideration that the individual’s predisposition toward field dependence and field independence is along a continuum, the median-split technique was applied. This method has also been used not only in previous studies on the interplay between FDI and creativity [36,38,47] but also in other research areas, including percep- tion [48], spatial cognition [46], and problem solving [49]. Participants were divided by the median-split of the EFT score (average solution times). Therefore, subjects with lower scores than the median (31.23) were classified as FIs (n = 30), whereas participants with higher scores than the median were classified as FDs (n = 30). Four Mann–Whitney U tests were performed in order to measure the differences between FDs and FIs in both preinventive and inventive phases. The significant level of the Mann–Whitney U tests was set as 5 % ( = 0.05). Results revealed that there are significant differences between FDs and FIs in terms of VCST Preinventive Originality (FDs rank = 23.83, FIs rank = 37.17, U = 250.000, p = 0.003), VCST Preinventive Synthesis (FDs rank = 23.47, FIs rank = 37.53, U = 239.000, p = 0.002), VCST Inventive Originality (FDs rank = 23.13, FIs rank = 37.87, U = 229.000, p = 0.001), and VCST Inventive Appropri- ateness (FDs rank = 23.35, FIs rank = 37.65, U = 235.000, p = 0.001). 4. Discussion Previous research on the relationships between FDI and creativity revealed unclear results, demonstrating a lack of consensus amongst researchers [21]: mixed findings have been found taking into account creative thinking in terms of convergent [37,38] and diver- gent productions [32,34], whereas little work has been done on creative production [39]. Given this scenario, the current research was aimed at investigating the extent to which the individual’s predisposition towards field dependence and field independence affects cre- ativity. To this aim, we measured FDI using the Embedded Figure Test, whereas creativity was evaluated by the VCST according to the logic of the Geneplore model. The VCST is a product-oriented task, which relies on preinventive and inventive phases. For the first time Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 8 of 12 in this research field, we adopted such a twofold model, assuming that creative produc- tion requires both ideas generation and ideas evaluation, underpinned by divergent and convergent mental processes, respectively [4]. Specifically, the VCST relies on a generative process in which individuals produce preinventive structures characterized by different degrees of creative potential and on an exploratory process in which such structures are evaluated by considering their potential fruition analytically [9]. Regarding the preinventive phase, results revealed that FI negatively correlated to originality and synthesis. This result was also confirmed by the ANOVA, showing that FIs provided significantly higher scores in originality and synthesis than FDs, meaning that the faster were participants in identifying the simple shape embedded in the complex figure (field independence), the more original and well-assembled were their preinventive structures. Thus, the H1 was confirmed. Given the nature of the task used, it is not surpris- ing that mental imagery plays a key role during the preinventive phase of VCST: indeed, the task requires mentally transforming, combining, and synthesizing visual components in order to generate preinventive structures, that is, mental prototypes of inventions. The pivotal role of mental imagery in creative tasks such as the VCST has been widely recog- nized in the past [10,50–52]. More specifically, spatial imagery and mental manipulation of spatial forms seem to be crucial in tasks involving objects’ construction [53], including creative inventions. Indeed, the ability to mentally manipulate shapes was found positively related with the originality score of preinventive structures [54] and the ability to generate shapes that were well-assembled and synthesized [10,55]. The role of spatial manipulation in creative tasks is also consistent with those studies using the think-aloud method in order to reveal mental processes actively involved in creativity. For instance, Palmiero and Piccardi’s study [56] revealed that spatial thoughts—containing spatial information of size and rotation—generated during the preinventive phase positively predicted the originality of productions during the invention phase. Although we did not detect mental imagery directly, the assumptions reported above could represent a relevant point to explain our results. Indeed, FIs seem to be more skilled than FDs in spatial abilities implying mental imagery. Specifically, FIs showed higher performance than FDs in tasks required to process different objects’ features such as shape and orientation [57] and in tasks tapping visual- spatial information [58]. For instance, Boccia and colleagues [59], in a sample of 50 young adults, found that FIs outperformed FDs in mental rotation test and Li and colleagues [60] revealed similar results in 2D and 3D map mental rotation, underlining that regardless of map dimensionality, as the degree of the image rotation increased, the accuracy of the FIs’ performance increased. Although the more flexible mental imagery and the better predisposition to use visual stimuli of FIs could represent a pivotal factor in this phase of the Geneplore cycle, undoubtedly, different mechanisms could affect it, and further investigations are needed. Regarding the inventive phase, results revealed that FDI negatively correlated with originality and appropriateness. This result was also confirmed by the ANOVA, showing that FIs provided significantly higher scores in originality and appropriateness than FDs, meaning that the faster participants were in identifying the simple shape embedded in the complex figure (field independence), the more creative (original and appropriate) were their inventions. Results confirmed H2 and align with previous studies on real-world creative production [29,39] as well as research on both divergent thinking [17,19,32] and convergent thinking [36,37]. Two main explanations can explain the better performance of FIs in the VCST than FDs. First, the assumption of the pivotal role of mental imagery in the preinventive phase can be also extended to the inventive phase. For instance, Roskos- ewoldsen and colleagues [54], in a sample composed of 41 young and 41 older adults, found a positive relationship between the Paper Folding Test and originality score of productions in the Creative Invention Task. Similar results were also found by Palmiero and colleagues’ study [51], in which the individual vividness of mental imagery was positively related to the practicality score of the invention in the Mental Synthesis Task. Therefore, the nature of the VCST used in this study and the better predisposition of FIs than FDs in mentally Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 9 of 12 manipulating spatial shapes could represent a possible explanation of our results during the inventive phase. Second, it has been found that the better predisposition of FIs in using their own knowledge and in extracting it from memory, especially in complex tasks in which the solution is unclear, positively affects creative performance [19]. This assumption is consistent with the two-step form of the VCST used in this research. Indeed, unlike the one- step form [29,51] in which the category of creative inventions is specified before combining the visual components, the category is specified in the two-step form only after the assembly of components. This makes the creative process more complex because, at least, in the combination phase, the goal of creative production is not defined, and participants have to adapt what they have previously assembled to the category provided by the task. In other words, during the two-step form of VCST, participants have to reorganize their prior knowledge in order to generate the creative product. Given that FIs, compared with FDs, have a better capacity to extract their own knowledge, this individual predisposition could be helpful to them in reorganizing and updating the structure previously generated in order to generate the creative invention. Taken together these results showed that field independence positively affected both phases (pre-inventive and inventive) of the creative process. Although one would be expected that FDI differently impacted on the two phases, it is important to acknowl- edge that, on the one hand, the preinventive phase relies on field independence based on divergent thinking [31,32]; and, on the other hand, the inventive phase relies on field independence based on convergent analytic thinking [36,37]. That is, FIs can encompass the creative process given their ability in shifting between generative/divergent and ex- plorative/convergent phases. This means that real-world creative production based on the Geneplore Model might be fully supported by the field independent cognitive style, given that the latter loads on both the preinventive and inventive phases. Although this interpretation is intriguing, it should be supported by further scientific evidence. Notably, the VCST—Description length correlated positively to the originality and appropriateness of the inventive scores, meaning that higher the number of words used to describe the objects the higher the originality and appropriateness. This result suggested that the description length may represent a characteristic of creativity [61], especially of the inventive phase of the creative process. Although in this study we did not used a written-narrative task to measure creativity, this result confirmed that when it is necessary to make causal inferences and category reductions, the number of words, reflecting the complexity of the construct [62], can play a key role. Of course, this idea needs also to be verified by future studies. 5. Conclusions To conclude, this research provides empirical evidence on a problematic and complex relationship involving FDI and creativity, and results seem to support the hypothesis that FIs outperform FDs in creative performance. Despite these findings, the current research shows a critical limit concerning the small sample size. This suggests that studies with more subjects should be carried out in order to reach more reliable conclusions. In addition, the field dependent and field independent groups are unbalanced in terms of gender. However, the study provides interesting future directions. First, although the Geneplore model represents a domain-general framework [11], the research focused only on visual real-world creative production. Further studies should encompass different domains of creativity (e.g., verbal and motor domains) in order to define an exhaustive picture of the impact of FDI on creativity. Yet, even though the VCST is not a time-based creativity task, future studies could assess the times necessary to carry on the creative process and related it to the attributes of creativity, such as originality and appropriateness. Then, in the present research, we stressed the pivotal role of mental imagery during the generation and evaluation of preinventive structure as well as during the invention phase. Future investigations could further explore the relationships amongst FDI, creativity, and mental Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 10 of 12 imagery, considering how the vividness of mental images may spur or inhibit this cognitive style’s influence on creative performance. Author Contributions: Conceptualization, M.G. and M.P.; methodology, M.G. and M.P.; formal analysis, M.G. and M.P.; investigation, M.G. and M.P.; resources, L.P. and S.D.; data curation, M.G.; writing—original draft preparation, M.G.; writing—review and editing, M.G., M.P., L.P. and S.D.; supervision, S.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript. Funding: This research received no external funding. Institutional Review Board Statement: The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethic Committee of the University of L’Aquila. Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Data Availability Statement: The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to subject confidentiality. Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest. References 1. Cropley, A.J. Creativity and mental health in everyday life. Creat. Res. J. 1990, 3, 167–178. [CrossRef] 2. Kaufman, J.C.; Beghetto, R.A. Beyond big and little: The four c model of creativity. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2009, 13, 1–12. [CrossRef] 3. Guilford, J.P. Creativity: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. J. Creat. Behav. 1967, 1, 3–14. [CrossRef] 4. Runco, M.A.; Acar, S. Divergent thinking as an indicator of creative potential. Creat. Res. J. 2012, 24, 66–75. [CrossRef] 5. Amabile, T.M. A model of creativity and innovation in organizations. Res. Organ. Behav. 1988, 10, 123–167. 6. Sternberg, R.J. A three-facet model of creativity. In The Nature of Creativity: Contemporary Psychological Perspectives; Sternberg, R.J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1988; pp. 125–147. 7. Finke, R.A.; Ward, T.B.; Smith, S.M. Creative Cognition: Theory, Research, and Applications; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 8. Gruszka, A.; Tang, M. The 4P’s creativity model and its application in different fields. In Handbook of the Management of Creativity and Innovation: Theory and Practice; Tang, M., Werner, C.H., Eds.; World Scientific Press: Singapore, 2017; pp. 51–71. 9. Ward, T.B. Creative cognition, conceptual combination, and the creative writing of Stephen R. Donaldson. Am. Psychol. 2001, 56, 350. [CrossRef] 10. Finke, R.A. Imagery, creativity, and emergent structure. Conscious. Cogn. 1996, 5, 381–393. [CrossRef] 11. Kleinmintz, O.M.; Ivancovsky, T.; Shamay-Tsoory, S.G. The two-fold model of creativity: The neural underpinnings of the generation and evaluation of creative ideas. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2019, 27, 131–138. [CrossRef] 12. Kaufman, J.C.; Sternberg, R.J. The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010. 13. Sternberg, R.J.; Lubart, T.I. An investment theory of creativity and its development. Hum. Dev. 1991, 34, 1–31. [CrossRef] 14. Abraham, A. The Neuroscience of Creativity; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2018. 15. Runco, M.A.; Jaeger, G.J. The standard definition of creativity. Creat. Res. J. 2012, 24, 92–96. [CrossRef] 16. Ward, T.B.; Kennedy, E.S. Creativity research: More studies, greater sophistication and the importance of “big” questions. J. Creat. Behav. 2017, 51, 285–288. [CrossRef] 17. Lei, W.; Deng, W.; Zhu, R.; Runco, M.A.; Dai, D.Y.; Hu, W. Does Cognitive Style Moderate Expected Evaluation and Adolescents’ Creative Performance: An Empirical Study. J. Creat. Behav. 2020, 55, 120–129. [CrossRef] 18. Riding, R.; Rayner, S. Cognitive Styles and Learning Strategies: Understanding Style Differences in Learning and Behavior; David Fulton Publishers: London, UK, 2013. 19. Li, C.; Mu, X.; Tan, Y.; Gu, C.; Hu, B.Y.; Fan, C. Do field-dependent individuals tend to have lower creativity than field-independent ones? The role of informational cues in electronic brainstorming. Interact. Learn. Environ. 2020, 28, 1–20. [CrossRef] 20. Witkin, H.A.; Moore, C.A.; Goodenough, D.R.; Cox, P.W. Field-dependent and field-independent cognitive styles and their educational implications. Rev. Educ. Res. 1977, 47, 1–64. [CrossRef] 21. Zhang, L.F. The Value of Intellectual Styles; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2017. 22. Zeng, X.Q.; Liu, J.P.; Chen, M.R. The relationship between cognitive style and implicit/explicit memory. J. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 33, 138–140. 23. Zhang, L.F. Field-dependence/independence: Cognitive style or perceptual ability? Validating against thinking styles and academic achievement. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2004, 37, 1295–1311. [CrossRef] 24. Guisande, M.A.; Páramo, M.F.; Tinajero, C.; Almeida, L.S. Field dependence-independence (FDI) cognitive style: An analysis of attentional functioning. Psicothema 2007, 19, 572–577. 25. Gerwig, A.; Miroshnik, K.; Forthmann, B.; Benedek, M.; Karwowski, M.; Holling, H. The relationship between intelligence and divergent thinking—A meta-analytic update. J. Intell. 2021, 9, 23. [CrossRef] Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 11 of 12 26. Palmiero, M.; Fusi, G.; Crepaldi, M.; Borsa, V.M.; Rusconi, M.L. Divergent thinking and the core executive functions: A state-of-the-art review. Cogn. Process. 2022. [CrossRef] 27. Lee, L.Y.; Talhelm, T.; Zhang, X.; Hu, B.; Lv, X. Holistic thinkers process divided-attention tasks faster: From the global/local perspective. Curr. Psychol. 2021. [CrossRef] 28. Reiter-Palmon, R.; Forthmann, B.; Barbot, B. Scoring divergent thinking tests: A review and systematic framework. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2019, 13, 144. [CrossRef] 29. Giancola, M.; Palmiero, M.; D’Amico, S. Exploring the interplay between fluid intelligence and creativity: The mediating role of the field-dependent-independent cognitive style. Think. Ski. Creat. 2022, 45, 101047. [CrossRef] 30. Giancola, M.; Palmiero, M.; D’Amico, S. Field dependent-independent cognitive style and creativity from the process and product-oriented approaches: A systematic review. Creat. Stud. 2007, 19, 572–577. 31. Spotts, J.V.; Mackler, B. Relationships of field-dependent and field-independent cognitive styles to creative test performance. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1967, 24, 239–268. [CrossRef] 32. Nisiforou, E.A. Examining the association between users creative thinking and field dependence-independence cognitive style through eye movement components. In Proceedings of the ACM SIGCHI Conference on Creativity and Cognition, Glasgow, UK, 22–25 June 2015. 33. Bloomberg, M. Creativity as related to field independence and mobility. J. Genet. Psychol. 1971, 118, 3–12. [CrossRef] 34. Niaz, M.; De Nunez, G.S.; De Pineda, I.R. Academic performance of high school students as a function of mental capacity, cognitive style, mobility-fixity dimension, and creativity. J. Creat. Behav. 2000, 34, 18–29. [CrossRef] 35. Bal, S. Creativity, cognitive style and academic achievement amongst university students. Psychol. Stud. 1988, 33, 10–13. 36. Noppe, L.D.; Gallagher, J.M. A cognitive style approach to creative thought. J. Pers. Assess. 1977, 41, 85–90. [CrossRef] 37. Chadha, N.K. Creativity and cognitive style. Psycho-Lingua 1985, 15, 81–88. 38. Ohnmacht, F.W.; McMorris, R.F. Creativity as a function of field independence and dogmatism. J. Psychol. 1971, 79, 165–168. [CrossRef] 39. Miller, A.L. Creativity and cognitive style: The relationship between field-dependence-independence, expected evaluation, and creative performance. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2007, 1, 243–246. [CrossRef] 40. Fergusson, L.C. Field independence and art achievement in meditating and nonmeditating college students. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1992, 75, 1171–1175. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 41. Fergusson, L.C. Field independence, transcendental meditation, and achievement in college art: A reexamination. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1993, 77, 1104–1106. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 42. Jaarsveld, S.; Lachmann, T. Intelligence and creativity in problem solving: The importance of test features in cognition research. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 134. [CrossRef] 43. Fogliani, T.; Di Nuovo, S.; Fogliani, A.M.; Pizzamiglio, L. Dipendenza Dal Campo e Stile Cognitivo: Gli Embedded Figures Tests Di H. Witkin, PK Oltman, E. Raskin e SA Karp; Organizzazioni Speciali: Firenze, Italy, 1984. 44. Palmiero, M.; Nori, R.; Aloisi, V.; Ferrara, M.; Piccardi, L. Domain-specificity of creativity: A study on the relationship between visual creativity and visual mental imagery. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1870. [CrossRef] 45. Amabile, T.M. Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 43, 997. [CrossRef] 46. Tascón, L.; Boccia, M.; Piccardi, L.; Cimadevilla, J.M. Differences in spatial memory recognition due to cognitive style. Front. Pharmacol. 2017, 8, 550. [CrossRef] 47. Niaz, M.; De Nunez, G.S. The relationship of mobility-fixity to creativity formal reasoning and intelligence. J. Creat. Behav. 1991, 25, 205–217. [CrossRef] 48. Teghil, A.; Boccia, M.; Guariglia, C. Field dependence–independence differently affects retrospective time estimation and flicker-induced time dilation. Exp. Brain Res. 2019, 237, 1019–1029. [CrossRef] 49. Mefoh, P.C.; Nwoke, M.B.; Chukwuorji, J.C.; Chijioke, A.O. Effect of cognitive style and gender on adolescents’ problem solving ability. Think. Ski. Creat. 2017, 25, 47–52. [CrossRef] 50. Palmiero, M.; Nakatani, C.; Raver, D.; Belardinelli, M.O.; van Leeuwen, C. Abilities within and across visual and verbal domains: How specific is their influence on creativity? Creat. Res. J. 2010, 22, 369–377. [CrossRef] 51. Palmiero, M.; Cardi, V.; Belardinelli, M.O. The role of vividness of visual mental imagery on different dimensions of creativity. Creat. Res. J. 2011, 23, 372–375. [CrossRef] 52. Palmiero, M.; Nori, R.; Piccardi, L. Visualizer cognitive style enhances visual creativity. Neurosci. Lett. 2016, 615, 98–101. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 53. Sack, A.T.; Jacobs, C.; De Martino, F.; Staeren, N.; Goebel, R.; Formisano, E. Dynamic premotor-to-parietal interactions during spatial imagery. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 8417–8429. [CrossRef] 54. Roskos-ewoldsen, B.; Black, S.R.; McCown, S.M. Age-related changes in creative thinking. J. Creat. Behav. 2008, 42, 33–59. [CrossRef] 55. Finke, R.A.; Pinker, S.; Farah, M.J. Reinterpreting visual patterns in mental imagery. Cogn. Sci. 1989, 13, 51–78. [CrossRef] 56. Palmiero, M.; Piccardi, L. Is Visual Creativity Embodied? Thinking Aloud while Performing the Creative Mental Synthesis Task. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 455. [CrossRef] 57. Hindal, H.; Reid, N.; Badgaish, M. Working memory, performance and learner characteristics. Res. Sci. Technol. 2009, 27, 187–204. [CrossRef] Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 12 of 12 58. Evans, C.; Richardson, J.T.; Waring, M. Field independence: Reviewing the evidence. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 83, 210–224. [CrossRef] 59. Boccia, M.; Piccardi, L.; Di Marco, M.; Pizzamiglio, L.; Guariglia, C. Does field independence predict visuo-spatial abilities underpinning human navigation? Behavioural evidence. Exp. Brain Res. 2016, 234, 2799–2807. [CrossRef] 60. Li, H.; Zhang, Y.; Wu, C.; Mei, D. Effects of field dependence-independence and frame of reference on navigation performance using multi-dimensional elec-tronic maps. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2016, 97, 289–299. [CrossRef] 61. Taylor, C.L.; Kaufman, J.C.; Barbot, B. Measuring creative writing with the storyboard task: The role of effort and story length. J. Creat. Behav. 2021, 55, 476–488. [CrossRef] 62. Burleson, B.R.; Samter, W.; Waltman, M.S. More evidence that cognitive complexity is not loquacity: A reply to beatty and payne. Commun. Q. 1987, 35, 317–328. [CrossRef] http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png Behavioral Sciences Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute

The Relationships between Cognitive Styles and Creativity: The Role of Field Dependence-Independence on Visual Creative Production

Loading next page...
 
/lp/multidisciplinary-digital-publishing-institute/the-relationships-between-cognitive-styles-and-creativity-the-role-of-MBZ5wE7c72

References

References for this paper are not available at this time. We will be adding them shortly, thank you for your patience.

Publisher
Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute
Copyright
© 1996-2022 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated Disclaimer The statements, opinions and data contained in the journals are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publisher and the editor(s). MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. Terms and Conditions Privacy Policy
ISSN
2076-328X
DOI
10.3390/bs12070212
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

behavioral sciences Article The Relationships between Cognitive Styles and Creativity: The Role of Field Dependence-Independence on Visual Creative Production 1 , 1 2 , 3 1 Marco Giancola * , Massimiliano Palmiero , Laura Piccardi and Simonetta D’Amico Department of Biotechnological and Applied Clinical Sciences, University of L’Aquila, 67100 L’Aquila, Italy; massimiliano.palmiero@univaq.it (M.P.); simonetta.damico@univaq.it (S.D.) Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, 00185 Rome, Italy; laura.piccardi@uniroma1.it Cognitive and Motor Rehabilitation and Neuroimaging Unit, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, 00179 Rome, Italy * Correspondence: marco.giancola@graduate.univaq.it Abstract: Previous studies explored the relationships between field dependent-independent cognitive style (FDI) and creativity, providing misleading and unclear results. The present research explored this problematic interplay through the lens of the Geneplore model, employing a product-oriented task: the Visual Creative Synthesis Task (VCST). The latter requires creating objects belonging to pre-established categories, starting from triads of visual components and consists of two steps: the preinventive phase and the inventive phase. Following the Amabile’s consensual assessment technique, three independent judges evaluated preinventive structures in terms of originality and synthesis whereas inventions were evaluated in terms of originality and appropriateness. The Em- bedded Figure Test (EFT) was employed in order to measure the individual’s predisposition toward the field dependence or the field independence. Sixty undergraduate college students (31 females) took part in the experiment. Results revealed that field independent individuals outperformed field dependent ones in each of the four VCST scores, showing higher levels of creativity. Results were Citation: Giancola, M.; Palmiero, M.; discussed in light of the better predisposition of field independent individuals in mental imagery, Piccardi, L.; D’Amico, S. The mental manipulation of abstract objects, as well as in using their knowledge during complex tasks Relationships between Cognitive that require creativity. Future research directions were also discussed. Styles and Creativity: The Role of Field Dependence-Independence on Keywords: field dependent-independent cognitive styles; creativity; creative production; Geneplore Visual Creative Production. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212. https://doi.org/ model; visual creative synthesis task 10.3390/bs12070212 Academic Editor: Scott D. Lane Received: 18 April 2022 1. Introduction Accepted: 22 June 2022 Creativity has been widely recognized as the key to success in contemporary society, Published: 25 June 2022 affecting art, science, economy, and everyday problem solving [1,2]. Given its relevance Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral in human activities, creativity has received growing attention since the second half of the with regard to jurisdictional claims in 20th century, when Guilford proposed the multifactorial Structure of Intellect Model [3], in published maps and institutional affil- which creative thinking encompassed convergent thinking (CT) and divergent thinking iations. (DT). Whereas CT is stated as the ability to converge on prevailing ways of thinking in order to find a single, right, and ready-made solution to a problem that other people could also reach, DT represents a spontaneous and free-flowing form of thought and exemplifies the ability to find many new solutions to an open-ended problem. Additionally, DT is Copyright: © 2022 by the authors. widely recognized as one of the main indicators of people’s creative potential [4], informing Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. about the likelihood that one can act “outside the box”. Although Guilford’s work has This article is an open access article represented a milestone in the literature of creativity, researchers have suggested alternative distributed under the terms and frameworks [5,6], including the Geneplore model [7]. The latter represents one of the most conditions of the Creative Commons influential developments in the tradition of cognitive psychology [8], and exemplifies Attribution (CC BY) license (https:// a twofold framework in which real-world creative production involves a cyclic motion creativecommons.org/licenses/by/ between generative and explorative phases. The generative phase plays a crucial role in 4.0/). Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12070212 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 2 of 12 producing pre-inventive structures that are internal prototypes of inventions characterized by different degrees of creative potential and originality [9]. Whereas this phase requires several cognitive resources, including memory retrieval, mental synthesis, mental trans- formation, and categorical reduction [10], the explorative phase, employing cognitive and meta-cognitive processes such as attribute finding, conceptual interpretation, functional inference, and hypothesis testing, drives the examination and practical interpretation of preinventive structures to generate a creative outcome [11]. According to the product perspective [12,13], two main criteria are necessary to evaluate creative inventions: origi- nality and appropriateness. Whereas the former encompasses the degree of novelty and uncommonness of productions, the latter refers to the relevance and usefulness, exemplify- ing the individual ability to produce an outcome that fits the needs and constraints of a given situation and is well situated in a given context [14]. Notably, even though different attributes for evaluating creative inventions can be found in the literature of creative pro- duction (e.g., elegance, aesthetics, and surprise), originality and appropriateness are the most accurate criteria, which reflect the full standard definition of creativity [15]. Given the multifaceted nature of creativity, the impact of cognitive factors on creative accomplishment has been long discussed in the past [16]. Cognitive style, also known as thinking style, represents a pivotal factor unquestionably related to creativity [17]. The expression cognitive style refers to how people acquire, organize, and use information [18]. Cognitive styles are usually conceptualized as bipolar, pervasive, and relatively stable over time, representing a critical dimension of the individual functioning. Although the key role of cognitive styles in human cognition and behavior has been extensively acknowledged, their universal explanatory power has not consistently been demonstrated. However, amongst all cognitive styles, the universal explanatory power of the field dependent- independent cognitive style (FDI) has been widely recognized [19]. In their seminal work, Witkin and colleagues defined FDI as “the extent to which the person perceives part of a field as discrete from the surrounding field as a whole, rather than embedded in the field” [20] (pp. 6–7). This cognitive style describes a stable and habitual tendency [21,22] characterized by two different poles: field dependence and field independence. Unlike field dependent subjects (FDs), field independent individuals (FIs) usually show less diffi- culty in separating information from the surrounding context [23] and are generally more focused on relevant information, inhibiting attention to irrelevant information coming from the environment [24]. Although FIs are generally defined as more flexible, open- minded, and capable of breaking down the routine than FDs, empirical evidence on the role of FDI on creativity provided unclear results [21], probably because of the involvement of specific cognitive processes (e.g., fluid intelligence, inhibition, working memory, and flexibility [25,26]), socio-cultural factors (e.g., Western vs. Eastern [27]), as well as the discrepancy between the scoring methods (e.g., empirically-based and rater-based scoring methods [28]), tasks used (e.g., divergent and convergent tasks, real-world creative produc- tion tasks [29]), and sampling bias. For a systematic review on the role of FDI in creativity, see Giancola et al. [30]. Regarding DT, some studies revealed that FIs outperformed FDs [31,32] in gener- ating ideas, whereas others found non-significant results [33,34]. For instance, Li and colleagues [19] revealed that FIs outperformed FDs in scientific and social brainstorming tasks in terms of fluency and novelty, confirming previous research [32,35]. In addition, Lei and colleagues [17] found that field independence was related to fluency and originality but not to flexibility, whereas Niaz and colleagues [34] found no significant effect of FDI. Regarding CT, some authors stressed that FIs attained significantly higher scores than FDs in convergent measures [36,37], others found no significant effect of FDI [38]. Finally, for real-world creative production, to the knowledge of the current research, only two studies explored the impact of FDI on creative production. Specifically, Miller ’s study [39] showed that FIs reported higher creativity scores than FDs in the creative collage making task. Similarly, Giancola et al. [29] found that FIs outperformed FDs in the ability to generate real-world creative objects. Notably, even though some authors found positive correlations Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 3 of 12 amongst FDI and self-rated artistic abilities and artistic competencies [40,41], research on the impact of FDI on creative production remains scattered to date. Therefore, the present research aimed to shed further light on the issue, employing the logic of the Geneplore model, which involves the combined effect of generation and exploration of ideas to generate real-world visual creative objects. Compared to Giancola et al. [29], who used a one-step procedure, priming participants with object category names while combining the visual stimuli, in the present study a two-step procedure was used. Specifically, participants were firstly instructed to construct pre-inventive forms combining the visual stimuli, and then interpret them within a specific conceptual category. This led to a better understanding of the role of FDI in the creative process, which encompasses both a visuo-spatial generative phase of undefined ideas and a conceptual/inferential phase of refined ideas. In this vein, the generative phase requires mainly divergent thinking to generate preinventive structures, whereas the explorative phase requires mainly convergent thinking to define and evaluate actual inventions [42]. Based on previous studies, the first two hypotheses were formulated as follows: Hypotheses 1 (H1). FIs outperform FDs in the preinventive phase because FIs are more divergent, thus more able in generating ideas than FDs [31,32]; Hypotheses 2 (H2). FIs outperform FDs in the inventive phase because FIs are also more conver- gent, thus more analytic in evaluating ideas than FDs [36,37]; Hypotheses 3 (H3). FIs do not outperform FDs, regardless of the phase of the creative process, because previous studies found inconsistent relationships between FDI and both divergent and convergent measures of the creative process [33,34,38]. 2. Materials and Methods 2.1. Participants and Procedure Sixty undergraduate college students attending different courses at The University of L’Aquila (L’Aquila, Italy) participated in the study (mean age = 22.30  3.31; age range = 19–32). Twenty-nine of them were males (48.3%), and thirty-one were females (51.7%). After signing the written informed consent to participate in the study, all participants were asked to complete an anamnesis questionnaire assessing biographical and educational information, general health state, and background or formal achievement in art. No participant reported psychiatric or neurological disorders, drug or alcohol addictions, and no participants declared a background or formal achievement in art. The experimental protocol was administered individually to each participant in a quiet room of the Socio- Cognitive Processes in Life Span Laboratory at The University of L’Aquila (L’Aquila, Italy). The experiment lasted approximately 45 min. The Local Ethics Committee approved this experiment in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. 2.2. Measures 2.2.1. Assessment of Field Dependent Independent Cognitive Style The Embedded Figure Test—EFT [43] is usually employed to evaluate the individual’s predisposition toward the field dependent or the field independent cognitive style. It is a paper and pencil test in which participants were requested to find a simple black and white shape within a geometric colored complex figure. The test consists of 24 cards (12 cards with simple shapes and 12 cards with complex figures) 12.9  7.7 cm (see Figure 1). Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 4 of 12 4 of 12 Figure 1. An example of an item taken from the Embedded Figure Test (EFT). (A) The geometric Figure 1. An example of an item taken from the Embedded Figure Test (EFT). (A) The geometric colored complex figure. (B) The simple black and white simple shape. (C) The simple shape within colored complex figure. (B) The simple black and white simple shape. (C) The simple shape within the complex figure. the complex figure. The experimenter presented the complex-colored figure one by one for 15 s and the The experimenter presented the complex-colored figure one by one for 15 s and the participant had to describe the figure in a loud voice. Then, the experimenter removed the participant had to describe the figure in a loud voice. Then, the experimenter removed the complex figure and presented the simple one; after 10 s, he took away the simple black and complex figure and presented the simple one; after 10 s, he took away the simple black white shape and presented once again the complex-colored figure. After that, participants and white shape and presented once again the complex-colored figure. After that, partic- had to find the simple black and white shape embedded in the complex figure. They were ipants had to find the simple black and white shape embedded in the complex figure. instructed to inform the experimenter as soon as they found the figure and trace its outlines They were instructed to inform the experimenter as soon as they found the figure and using a pencil. When the participants declared to have found the simple black and white trace its outlines using a pencil. When the participants declared to have found the simple shape within the complex figure, the experimenter annotated the elapsed time (timing). If black and white shape within the complex figure, the experimenter annotated the elapsed the response (tracing of the outlines) was wrong, the experimenter continued to take the time until the participant provided the correct response or until 180 s had elapsed. The total time (timing). If the response (tracing of the outlines) was wrong, the experimenter con- time was divided by the number of items (12) in order to compute the average time (RTs) tinued to take the time until the participant provided the correct response or until 180 s which was used as the measure of the individual’s cognitive style. A shorter time indicated had elapsed. The total time was divided by the number of items (12) in order to compute a higher predisposition towards field independence, whereas a longer time indicated a the average time (RTs) which was used as the measure of the individual’s cognitive style. higher predisposition towards field dependence. A shorter time indicated a higher predisposition towards field independence, whereas a longer time indicated a higher predisposition towards field dependence. 2.2.2. Assessment of Creativity The Visual Creative Synthesis Task—VCST [7] aimed to create objects belonging to 2.2.2. Assessment of Creativity pre-established categories, starting from triads of visual components. The task consists of two steps: the preinventive phase and the inventive phase. Following Palmiero and The Visual Creative Synthesis Task—VCST [7] aimed to create objects belonging to colleagues [44], three triads of components and three categories were used. Preinventive pre-established categories, starting from triads of visual components. The task consists of phase: participants, after a practical example, were asked to combine the components into a two steps: the preinventive phase and the inventive phase. Following Palmiero and col- preinventive structure, one for each triad. They could be changed in position, rotation, and leagues [44], three triads of components and three categories were used. Preinventive size but not in their general structure. The triads of components were presented along with phase: participants, after a practical example, were asked to combine the components into their name on a paper sheet (see Figure 2). Participants had 15 s to fix and memorize the a preinventive structure, one for each triad. They could be changed in position, rotation, components and 2 min to think of the preinventive structure for each triad. Inventive phase: and size but not after in thcr eieating r genethe ral thr stree ucpr tur einventive e. The tristr aductur s of c es, om participants ponents wwer ere eppr reesented sented with along a category name for each of them (1 furniture, 1 weapon, and 1 sport goods) and were requested to with their name on a paper sheet (see Figure 2). Participants had 15 s to fix and memorize think of their inventions. Participants had 3 min to describe the functioning of inventions the components and 2 min to think of the preinventive structure for each triad. Inventive (1 min for each invention), also providing their title (see Figure 3). The description of the phase: after creating the three preinventive structures, participants were presented with a objects was also defined in terms of length of responses (i.e., number of words). The order category name for each of them (1 furniture, 1 weapon, and 1 sport goods) and were re- of the triads was randomized. quested to think of their inventions. Participants had 3 min to describe the functioning of inventions (1 min for each invention), also providing their title (see Figure 3). The descrip- tion of the objects was also defined in terms of length of responses (i.e., number of words). The order of the triads was randomized. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 5 of 12 Beh Behav av. .SSci. ci. 2 2022 022,, 112 2,, 2 212 12 5 of 12 5 of 12 Figure 2. The three triads of components for the Visual Creative Synthesis Task (VCST): (1) cube, Figure 2. The three triads of components for the Visual Creative Synthesis Task (VCST): (1) cube, braF cig ke u tr , ec on 2. Th e (e s p tor hree t g t ood riad ss ); of (2 c ) om pap ra on llel en ep ts ip for ed t,h d e y V -p isy ur aa l m Cird e,a th iv or e n S y (fu nth rn es itiu s rTa e); s k (3 (V ) s C tr SiT): p, t(r 1a ) p cez ub oi e,d , cyl bracket, b in rd ac er ke (w t,cone c eon ape o (sport n (ss p ). or goods); t goods); (2 ) (2 parallelepiped, ) parallelepiped dy-pyramid, , dy-pyrami horn d, hor (furnitur n (furne); itur (3 e); ) strip, (3) sttrapezoid, rip, trapezoid, cylinder (weapons). cylinder (weapons). Figure 3. An example of creative production based on the triad n.3 made up of one stripe, one Figure 3. An example of creative production based on the triad n.3 made up of one stripe, one trap- trapezoid, and one cylinder. Category: Weapons; Title: Grenade; Description: This cylinder is a Figure 3. An example of creative production based on the triad n.3 made up of one stripe, one trap- ezoid, and one cylinder. Category: Weapons; Title: Grenade; Description: This cylinder is a grenade. grenade. The cylinder contains the explosive, the trapezoid is the trigger mechanism, and the strip ezoid, and one cylinder. Category: Weapons; Title: Grenade; Description: This cylinder is a grenade. The cylinder contains the explosive, the trapezoid is the trigger mechanism, and the strip controls contr The ols cylithe nder whole contgr ain enade s the and explhas osiv ae, safety the tr function. apezoid is the trigger mechanism, and the strip controls the whole grenade and has a safety function. the whole grenade and has a safety function. Based on Amabile’s consensual assessment technique [45], three independent judges, two Ba females sed on and Ama one bile male ’s co(mean nsensua age l a= ss25.33 essm en4.50), t techevaluated nique [45]pr , th einventive ree indep str en uctur dent es judges, Based on Amabile’s consensual assessment technique [45], three independent judges, and inventions. The judges were three psychology students who attended training on two females and one male (mean age = 25.33 ± 4.50), evaluated preinventive structures two females and one male (mean age = 25.33 ± 4.50), evaluated preinventive structures creativity and its assessment for a total of 20 h. During the training sessions, the main and inventions. The judges were three psychology students who attended training on cre- and inventions. The judges were three psychology students who attended training on cre- models and descriptive frameworks of creativity were explained, including the SOI and ativ ati ity vi ty an a dn id ts i ta ss a ss es se ss m sm en et nft o fr o r a a to to ta ta l lo o f f2 2 00 h h . .D Dur uriin ng g tth he e t tr ra ai in niin ng g s se essssiio on n ss , ,th th ee m m aa in in m m od od ele sl s the Geneplore Model. In addition, students were shown examples of creative productions and an d d ed sc er sic p rti ip v ti ev e fr a fr m am ew ew or ok rs k so o f fc r ce re aa titi vv it it y y w we erre e e ex xp plla ai in ne ed d,, iin nc cllud udiin n g g tt hh ee SS O O I I an an dd th th e e already evaluated in the past by judges, and they were trained to evaluate creative pro- Geneplore Model. In addition, students were shown examples of creative productions al- Geneplore Model. In addition, students were shown examples of creative productions al- ductions in terms of creativity. After the training, the evaluation sessions began. Notably, ready evaluated in the past by judges, and they were trained to evaluate creative produc- ready evaluated in the past by judges, and they were trained to evaluate creative produc- three basic parameters were used: originality and appropriateness, which are the most tions in terms of creativity. After the training, the evaluation sessions began. Notably, tions in terms of creativity. After the training, the evaluation sessions began. Notably, accurate and used criteria, reflecting the full standard definition of creativity [15], as well three basic parameters were used: originality and appropriateness, which are the most thr as ee synthesis, basic pawhich ramete was rs w used ere specifically used: origto inevaluate ality and the ap participants’ propriatene ability ss, wto hic holistically h are the most accurate and used criteria, reflecting the full standard definition of creativity [15], as well associate the elements during the preinventive phase of the VCST. Therefore, the preinven- accurate and used criteria, reflecting the full standard definition of creativity [15], as well as synthesis, which was used specifically to evaluate the participants’ ability to holistically tive structures were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms of as synthesis, which was used specifically to evaluate the participants’ ability to holistically originality associate ,th de efined elem as en ats form durbeing ing thnew e prand einvnot enti derived ve phas fr eom of something the VCST.else The (fr reom fore 1, = th very e prein- associate the elements during the preinventive phase of the VCST. Therefore, the prein- poor originality to 5 = very high originality) and synthesis, defined as the extent to which ventive structures were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms ventive structures were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms components were well assembled (from 1 = very poor synthesis to 5 = very high synthesis). of originality, defined as a form being new and not derived from something else (from 1 of originality, defined as a form being new and not derived from something else (from 1 The inter-rater correlation (intra-class correlation coefficient—absolute agreement) were = very poor originality to 5 = very high originality) and synthesis, defined as the extent to = very poor originality to 5 = very high originality) and synthesis, defined as the extent to significant for both originality (a = 0.92; p < 0.01) and synthesis (a = 0.93; p < 0.01). Inven- which components were well assembled (from 1 = very poor synthesis to 5 = very high which components were well assembled (from 1 = very poor synthesis to 5 = very high tions were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms of originality synthesis). The inter-rater correlation (intra-class correlation coefficient—absolute agree- sydefined nthesis)as . T ahpr e oduct inter-r being ater c new orreand latio not n (derived intra-cla fr som s co something rrelation c else oeff (fr icom ient 1— =a very bsolpoor ute agree- ment) were significant for both originality (α = 0.92; p < 0.01) and synthesis (α = 0.93; p < originality to 5 = very high originality), and appropriateness, defined as an invention with a ment) were significant for both originality (α = 0.92; p < 0.01) and synthesis (α = 0.93; p < 0.01). Inventions were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms practical instead of a hypothetical use (from 1 = very poor appropriateness to 5 = very high 0.01). Inventions were evaluated by each judge along a 5-point Likert-type scale in terms of originality defined as a product being new and not derived from something else (from of originality defined as a product being new and not derived from something else (from 1 = very poor originality to 5 = very high originality), and appropriateness, defined as an 1 = very poor originality to 5 = very high originality), and appropriateness, defined as an invention with a practical instead of a hypothetical use (from 1 = very poor appropriate- invention with a practical instead of a hypothetical use (from 1 = very poor appropriate- ness to 5 = very high appropriateness). The inter-rater correlation (intra-class correlation ness to 5 = very high appropriateness). The inter-rater correlation (intra-class correlation Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 6 of 12 appropriateness). The inter-rater correlation (intra-class correlation coefficient—absolute agreement) was significant for both originality (a = 0.94; p < 0.01) and appropriateness (a = 0.96; p < 0.01). 3. Results Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 24 for Windows (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). Data were tested for normality and all measures were normally distributed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov Test: Z VCST—Pre—Originality = 0.75, ns; Z VCST—Pre—Synthesis = 0.73, ns; Z VCST—Inv—Originality = 0.76, ns; Z VCST— Inv—Appropriateness = 0.31, ns; Z — = 0.84, ns) except for the EFT VCST Description length (RTs): (Kolmogorov–Smirnov Test: Z = 0.02, sig). Additionally, the analysis of the EFT (RTs) distribution of the EFT using the interquartile range method (IQR) revealed that only four FDs were ‘far out’ from the mean. This suggests that in the present sample data were mostly skewed toward field dependence (e.g., high RTs) rather than field independence (e.g., low RTs). Table 1 reported descriptive statistics divided for group (field dependence vs. field independence). Table 1. Descriptive statistics divided by FDI groups. FDs FIs N 30 30 Gender 21 F 10 F Age mean (SD) 21.61 (3.10) 22.41 (3.56) VCST—Pre—Originality mean (SD) 1.86 (0.37) 2.18 (0.41) VCST—Pre—Synthesis mean (SD) 1.76 (0.49) 2.25 (0.56) VCST—Inv—Originality mean (SD) 2.14 (0.59) 2.79 (0.76) VCST—Inv—Appropriateness mean (SD) 1.97 (0.66) 2.67 (0.81) VCST—Description length 44.33 (25.66) 59.10 (28.43) Note. FDs = Field Dependents; FIs = Field Independents; VCST = Visual Creative Synthesis Task; Pre = Preinven- tive phase; Inv = Inventive phase. Correlational analysis was computed using Spearman’s Rho (see Table 2). Results revealed that the EFT (RTs) was negatively correlated with the VCST—Preinventive phase Originality (r = 0.50, p < 0.01), VCST—Preinventive phase Synthesis (r = 0.48, p < 0.01), VCST—Inventive phase Originality (r = 0.51, p < 0.01) VCST—Inventive phase Appropri- ateness (r = 0.52, p < 0.01), and VCST—Description length (r = 0.30, p < 0.05). Notably, VCST—Description length positively correlated to VCST—Inventive phase Originality (r = 0.31, p < 0.05) VCST—Inventive phase Appropriateness (r = 0.29, p < 0.05). In addi- tion, gender also correlated negatively to EFT (RTs) (r = 0.44, p < 0.01) and positively to VCST—Preinventive phase Originality (r = 0.34, p < 0.01), VCST—Preinventive phase Synthesis (r = 0.35, p < 0.01), VCST—Inventive phase Originality (r = 0.27, p < 0.05) and VCST—Inventive phase Appropriateness (r = 0.29, p < 0.05), meaning that males showed higher field dependence and creativity scores than females. In order to obtain a more accurate picture of the relationship between EFT-RTs and the creativity scores, possible gender confounding effects were checked using the Spearman’s Rho partial correlations (see Table 3). The latter showed that controlling for gender had a little effect on the strength of the relationships between EFT-RTs and the creativity scores, and the significance level was not affected at all. Therefore, the variable ‘gender ’ was not further considered when analyzing the comparison between FDs and FIs in terms of creativity. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 7 of 12 Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and Spearman’s Rho inter-correlations. M SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 1. Gender - - 1 2. EFT (RTs) 41.63 29.25 0.44 ** 1 3. VCST—Pre—Originality 2.02 0.42 0.34 ** 0.50 ** 1 4. VCST—Pre—Synthesis 2.01 0.57 0.35 ** 0.48 ** 0.91 ** 1 5. VCST—Inv—Originality 2.47 0.74 0.27 * 0.51 ** 0.86 ** 0.84 ** 1 6. VCST—Inv—Appropriateness 2.34 0.81 0.29 * 0.52 ** 0.82 ** 0.85 ** 0.93 ** 1 7. VCST—Description length 52.02 27.79 0.02 0.30 * 0.15 0.15 0.31 * 0.29 * 1 Note. N = 60, gender was dummy coded (F = 0, M = 1) * p < 0.05 (two tailed) ** p < 0.01 (two tailed) EFT = Embedded Figure Test RTs = Response Times: VCST = Visual Creative Synthesis Task Pre = Preinventive phase Inv = Inventive phase. Table 3. Spearman’s Rho partial correlations. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 1. EFT (RTs) 2. VCST—Pre—Originality 0.41 ** 1 3. VCST—Pre—Synthesis 0.38 ** 0.90 ** 1 4. VCST—Inv—Originality 0.46 ** 0.85 ** 0.83 ** 1 5. VCST—Inv—Appropriateness 0.46 ** 0.80 ** 0.84 ** 0.93 ** 1 6. VCST—Description length 0.35 ** 0.16 0.17 0.33 * 0.31 * 1 Note. N = 60, * p < 0.05 (two tailed) ** p < 0.01 (two tailed) EFT = Embedded Figure Test RTs = Response Times: VCST = Visual Creative Synthesis Task Pre = Preinventive phase Inv = Inventive phase. Following Tascón and colleagues [46], since the EFT does not have a scale to divide FIs and FDs and taking into consideration that the individual’s predisposition toward field dependence and field independence is along a continuum, the median-split technique was applied. This method has also been used not only in previous studies on the interplay between FDI and creativity [36,38,47] but also in other research areas, including percep- tion [48], spatial cognition [46], and problem solving [49]. Participants were divided by the median-split of the EFT score (average solution times). Therefore, subjects with lower scores than the median (31.23) were classified as FIs (n = 30), whereas participants with higher scores than the median were classified as FDs (n = 30). Four Mann–Whitney U tests were performed in order to measure the differences between FDs and FIs in both preinventive and inventive phases. The significant level of the Mann–Whitney U tests was set as 5 % ( = 0.05). Results revealed that there are significant differences between FDs and FIs in terms of VCST Preinventive Originality (FDs rank = 23.83, FIs rank = 37.17, U = 250.000, p = 0.003), VCST Preinventive Synthesis (FDs rank = 23.47, FIs rank = 37.53, U = 239.000, p = 0.002), VCST Inventive Originality (FDs rank = 23.13, FIs rank = 37.87, U = 229.000, p = 0.001), and VCST Inventive Appropri- ateness (FDs rank = 23.35, FIs rank = 37.65, U = 235.000, p = 0.001). 4. Discussion Previous research on the relationships between FDI and creativity revealed unclear results, demonstrating a lack of consensus amongst researchers [21]: mixed findings have been found taking into account creative thinking in terms of convergent [37,38] and diver- gent productions [32,34], whereas little work has been done on creative production [39]. Given this scenario, the current research was aimed at investigating the extent to which the individual’s predisposition towards field dependence and field independence affects cre- ativity. To this aim, we measured FDI using the Embedded Figure Test, whereas creativity was evaluated by the VCST according to the logic of the Geneplore model. The VCST is a product-oriented task, which relies on preinventive and inventive phases. For the first time Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 8 of 12 in this research field, we adopted such a twofold model, assuming that creative produc- tion requires both ideas generation and ideas evaluation, underpinned by divergent and convergent mental processes, respectively [4]. Specifically, the VCST relies on a generative process in which individuals produce preinventive structures characterized by different degrees of creative potential and on an exploratory process in which such structures are evaluated by considering their potential fruition analytically [9]. Regarding the preinventive phase, results revealed that FI negatively correlated to originality and synthesis. This result was also confirmed by the ANOVA, showing that FIs provided significantly higher scores in originality and synthesis than FDs, meaning that the faster were participants in identifying the simple shape embedded in the complex figure (field independence), the more original and well-assembled were their preinventive structures. Thus, the H1 was confirmed. Given the nature of the task used, it is not surpris- ing that mental imagery plays a key role during the preinventive phase of VCST: indeed, the task requires mentally transforming, combining, and synthesizing visual components in order to generate preinventive structures, that is, mental prototypes of inventions. The pivotal role of mental imagery in creative tasks such as the VCST has been widely recog- nized in the past [10,50–52]. More specifically, spatial imagery and mental manipulation of spatial forms seem to be crucial in tasks involving objects’ construction [53], including creative inventions. Indeed, the ability to mentally manipulate shapes was found positively related with the originality score of preinventive structures [54] and the ability to generate shapes that were well-assembled and synthesized [10,55]. The role of spatial manipulation in creative tasks is also consistent with those studies using the think-aloud method in order to reveal mental processes actively involved in creativity. For instance, Palmiero and Piccardi’s study [56] revealed that spatial thoughts—containing spatial information of size and rotation—generated during the preinventive phase positively predicted the originality of productions during the invention phase. Although we did not detect mental imagery directly, the assumptions reported above could represent a relevant point to explain our results. Indeed, FIs seem to be more skilled than FDs in spatial abilities implying mental imagery. Specifically, FIs showed higher performance than FDs in tasks required to process different objects’ features such as shape and orientation [57] and in tasks tapping visual- spatial information [58]. For instance, Boccia and colleagues [59], in a sample of 50 young adults, found that FIs outperformed FDs in mental rotation test and Li and colleagues [60] revealed similar results in 2D and 3D map mental rotation, underlining that regardless of map dimensionality, as the degree of the image rotation increased, the accuracy of the FIs’ performance increased. Although the more flexible mental imagery and the better predisposition to use visual stimuli of FIs could represent a pivotal factor in this phase of the Geneplore cycle, undoubtedly, different mechanisms could affect it, and further investigations are needed. Regarding the inventive phase, results revealed that FDI negatively correlated with originality and appropriateness. This result was also confirmed by the ANOVA, showing that FIs provided significantly higher scores in originality and appropriateness than FDs, meaning that the faster participants were in identifying the simple shape embedded in the complex figure (field independence), the more creative (original and appropriate) were their inventions. Results confirmed H2 and align with previous studies on real-world creative production [29,39] as well as research on both divergent thinking [17,19,32] and convergent thinking [36,37]. Two main explanations can explain the better performance of FIs in the VCST than FDs. First, the assumption of the pivotal role of mental imagery in the preinventive phase can be also extended to the inventive phase. For instance, Roskos- ewoldsen and colleagues [54], in a sample composed of 41 young and 41 older adults, found a positive relationship between the Paper Folding Test and originality score of productions in the Creative Invention Task. Similar results were also found by Palmiero and colleagues’ study [51], in which the individual vividness of mental imagery was positively related to the practicality score of the invention in the Mental Synthesis Task. Therefore, the nature of the VCST used in this study and the better predisposition of FIs than FDs in mentally Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 9 of 12 manipulating spatial shapes could represent a possible explanation of our results during the inventive phase. Second, it has been found that the better predisposition of FIs in using their own knowledge and in extracting it from memory, especially in complex tasks in which the solution is unclear, positively affects creative performance [19]. This assumption is consistent with the two-step form of the VCST used in this research. Indeed, unlike the one- step form [29,51] in which the category of creative inventions is specified before combining the visual components, the category is specified in the two-step form only after the assembly of components. This makes the creative process more complex because, at least, in the combination phase, the goal of creative production is not defined, and participants have to adapt what they have previously assembled to the category provided by the task. In other words, during the two-step form of VCST, participants have to reorganize their prior knowledge in order to generate the creative product. Given that FIs, compared with FDs, have a better capacity to extract their own knowledge, this individual predisposition could be helpful to them in reorganizing and updating the structure previously generated in order to generate the creative invention. Taken together these results showed that field independence positively affected both phases (pre-inventive and inventive) of the creative process. Although one would be expected that FDI differently impacted on the two phases, it is important to acknowl- edge that, on the one hand, the preinventive phase relies on field independence based on divergent thinking [31,32]; and, on the other hand, the inventive phase relies on field independence based on convergent analytic thinking [36,37]. That is, FIs can encompass the creative process given their ability in shifting between generative/divergent and ex- plorative/convergent phases. This means that real-world creative production based on the Geneplore Model might be fully supported by the field independent cognitive style, given that the latter loads on both the preinventive and inventive phases. Although this interpretation is intriguing, it should be supported by further scientific evidence. Notably, the VCST—Description length correlated positively to the originality and appropriateness of the inventive scores, meaning that higher the number of words used to describe the objects the higher the originality and appropriateness. This result suggested that the description length may represent a characteristic of creativity [61], especially of the inventive phase of the creative process. Although in this study we did not used a written-narrative task to measure creativity, this result confirmed that when it is necessary to make causal inferences and category reductions, the number of words, reflecting the complexity of the construct [62], can play a key role. Of course, this idea needs also to be verified by future studies. 5. Conclusions To conclude, this research provides empirical evidence on a problematic and complex relationship involving FDI and creativity, and results seem to support the hypothesis that FIs outperform FDs in creative performance. Despite these findings, the current research shows a critical limit concerning the small sample size. This suggests that studies with more subjects should be carried out in order to reach more reliable conclusions. In addition, the field dependent and field independent groups are unbalanced in terms of gender. However, the study provides interesting future directions. First, although the Geneplore model represents a domain-general framework [11], the research focused only on visual real-world creative production. Further studies should encompass different domains of creativity (e.g., verbal and motor domains) in order to define an exhaustive picture of the impact of FDI on creativity. Yet, even though the VCST is not a time-based creativity task, future studies could assess the times necessary to carry on the creative process and related it to the attributes of creativity, such as originality and appropriateness. Then, in the present research, we stressed the pivotal role of mental imagery during the generation and evaluation of preinventive structure as well as during the invention phase. Future investigations could further explore the relationships amongst FDI, creativity, and mental Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 10 of 12 imagery, considering how the vividness of mental images may spur or inhibit this cognitive style’s influence on creative performance. Author Contributions: Conceptualization, M.G. and M.P.; methodology, M.G. and M.P.; formal analysis, M.G. and M.P.; investigation, M.G. and M.P.; resources, L.P. and S.D.; data curation, M.G.; writing—original draft preparation, M.G.; writing—review and editing, M.G., M.P., L.P. and S.D.; supervision, S.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript. Funding: This research received no external funding. Institutional Review Board Statement: The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethic Committee of the University of L’Aquila. Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Data Availability Statement: The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to subject confidentiality. Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest. References 1. Cropley, A.J. Creativity and mental health in everyday life. Creat. Res. J. 1990, 3, 167–178. [CrossRef] 2. Kaufman, J.C.; Beghetto, R.A. Beyond big and little: The four c model of creativity. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2009, 13, 1–12. [CrossRef] 3. Guilford, J.P. Creativity: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. J. Creat. Behav. 1967, 1, 3–14. [CrossRef] 4. Runco, M.A.; Acar, S. Divergent thinking as an indicator of creative potential. Creat. Res. J. 2012, 24, 66–75. [CrossRef] 5. Amabile, T.M. A model of creativity and innovation in organizations. Res. Organ. Behav. 1988, 10, 123–167. 6. Sternberg, R.J. A three-facet model of creativity. In The Nature of Creativity: Contemporary Psychological Perspectives; Sternberg, R.J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1988; pp. 125–147. 7. Finke, R.A.; Ward, T.B.; Smith, S.M. Creative Cognition: Theory, Research, and Applications; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 8. Gruszka, A.; Tang, M. The 4P’s creativity model and its application in different fields. In Handbook of the Management of Creativity and Innovation: Theory and Practice; Tang, M., Werner, C.H., Eds.; World Scientific Press: Singapore, 2017; pp. 51–71. 9. Ward, T.B. Creative cognition, conceptual combination, and the creative writing of Stephen R. Donaldson. Am. Psychol. 2001, 56, 350. [CrossRef] 10. Finke, R.A. Imagery, creativity, and emergent structure. Conscious. Cogn. 1996, 5, 381–393. [CrossRef] 11. Kleinmintz, O.M.; Ivancovsky, T.; Shamay-Tsoory, S.G. The two-fold model of creativity: The neural underpinnings of the generation and evaluation of creative ideas. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2019, 27, 131–138. [CrossRef] 12. Kaufman, J.C.; Sternberg, R.J. The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010. 13. Sternberg, R.J.; Lubart, T.I. An investment theory of creativity and its development. Hum. Dev. 1991, 34, 1–31. [CrossRef] 14. Abraham, A. The Neuroscience of Creativity; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2018. 15. Runco, M.A.; Jaeger, G.J. The standard definition of creativity. Creat. Res. J. 2012, 24, 92–96. [CrossRef] 16. Ward, T.B.; Kennedy, E.S. Creativity research: More studies, greater sophistication and the importance of “big” questions. J. Creat. Behav. 2017, 51, 285–288. [CrossRef] 17. Lei, W.; Deng, W.; Zhu, R.; Runco, M.A.; Dai, D.Y.; Hu, W. Does Cognitive Style Moderate Expected Evaluation and Adolescents’ Creative Performance: An Empirical Study. J. Creat. Behav. 2020, 55, 120–129. [CrossRef] 18. Riding, R.; Rayner, S. Cognitive Styles and Learning Strategies: Understanding Style Differences in Learning and Behavior; David Fulton Publishers: London, UK, 2013. 19. Li, C.; Mu, X.; Tan, Y.; Gu, C.; Hu, B.Y.; Fan, C. Do field-dependent individuals tend to have lower creativity than field-independent ones? The role of informational cues in electronic brainstorming. Interact. Learn. Environ. 2020, 28, 1–20. [CrossRef] 20. Witkin, H.A.; Moore, C.A.; Goodenough, D.R.; Cox, P.W. Field-dependent and field-independent cognitive styles and their educational implications. Rev. Educ. Res. 1977, 47, 1–64. [CrossRef] 21. Zhang, L.F. The Value of Intellectual Styles; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2017. 22. Zeng, X.Q.; Liu, J.P.; Chen, M.R. The relationship between cognitive style and implicit/explicit memory. J. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 33, 138–140. 23. Zhang, L.F. Field-dependence/independence: Cognitive style or perceptual ability? Validating against thinking styles and academic achievement. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2004, 37, 1295–1311. [CrossRef] 24. Guisande, M.A.; Páramo, M.F.; Tinajero, C.; Almeida, L.S. Field dependence-independence (FDI) cognitive style: An analysis of attentional functioning. Psicothema 2007, 19, 572–577. 25. Gerwig, A.; Miroshnik, K.; Forthmann, B.; Benedek, M.; Karwowski, M.; Holling, H. The relationship between intelligence and divergent thinking—A meta-analytic update. J. Intell. 2021, 9, 23. [CrossRef] Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 11 of 12 26. Palmiero, M.; Fusi, G.; Crepaldi, M.; Borsa, V.M.; Rusconi, M.L. Divergent thinking and the core executive functions: A state-of-the-art review. Cogn. Process. 2022. [CrossRef] 27. Lee, L.Y.; Talhelm, T.; Zhang, X.; Hu, B.; Lv, X. Holistic thinkers process divided-attention tasks faster: From the global/local perspective. Curr. Psychol. 2021. [CrossRef] 28. Reiter-Palmon, R.; Forthmann, B.; Barbot, B. Scoring divergent thinking tests: A review and systematic framework. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2019, 13, 144. [CrossRef] 29. Giancola, M.; Palmiero, M.; D’Amico, S. Exploring the interplay between fluid intelligence and creativity: The mediating role of the field-dependent-independent cognitive style. Think. Ski. Creat. 2022, 45, 101047. [CrossRef] 30. Giancola, M.; Palmiero, M.; D’Amico, S. Field dependent-independent cognitive style and creativity from the process and product-oriented approaches: A systematic review. Creat. Stud. 2007, 19, 572–577. 31. Spotts, J.V.; Mackler, B. Relationships of field-dependent and field-independent cognitive styles to creative test performance. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1967, 24, 239–268. [CrossRef] 32. Nisiforou, E.A. Examining the association between users creative thinking and field dependence-independence cognitive style through eye movement components. In Proceedings of the ACM SIGCHI Conference on Creativity and Cognition, Glasgow, UK, 22–25 June 2015. 33. Bloomberg, M. Creativity as related to field independence and mobility. J. Genet. Psychol. 1971, 118, 3–12. [CrossRef] 34. Niaz, M.; De Nunez, G.S.; De Pineda, I.R. Academic performance of high school students as a function of mental capacity, cognitive style, mobility-fixity dimension, and creativity. J. Creat. Behav. 2000, 34, 18–29. [CrossRef] 35. Bal, S. Creativity, cognitive style and academic achievement amongst university students. Psychol. Stud. 1988, 33, 10–13. 36. Noppe, L.D.; Gallagher, J.M. A cognitive style approach to creative thought. J. Pers. Assess. 1977, 41, 85–90. [CrossRef] 37. Chadha, N.K. Creativity and cognitive style. Psycho-Lingua 1985, 15, 81–88. 38. Ohnmacht, F.W.; McMorris, R.F. Creativity as a function of field independence and dogmatism. J. Psychol. 1971, 79, 165–168. [CrossRef] 39. Miller, A.L. Creativity and cognitive style: The relationship between field-dependence-independence, expected evaluation, and creative performance. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2007, 1, 243–246. [CrossRef] 40. Fergusson, L.C. Field independence and art achievement in meditating and nonmeditating college students. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1992, 75, 1171–1175. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 41. Fergusson, L.C. Field independence, transcendental meditation, and achievement in college art: A reexamination. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1993, 77, 1104–1106. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 42. Jaarsveld, S.; Lachmann, T. Intelligence and creativity in problem solving: The importance of test features in cognition research. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 134. [CrossRef] 43. Fogliani, T.; Di Nuovo, S.; Fogliani, A.M.; Pizzamiglio, L. Dipendenza Dal Campo e Stile Cognitivo: Gli Embedded Figures Tests Di H. Witkin, PK Oltman, E. Raskin e SA Karp; Organizzazioni Speciali: Firenze, Italy, 1984. 44. Palmiero, M.; Nori, R.; Aloisi, V.; Ferrara, M.; Piccardi, L. Domain-specificity of creativity: A study on the relationship between visual creativity and visual mental imagery. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1870. [CrossRef] 45. Amabile, T.M. Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 43, 997. [CrossRef] 46. Tascón, L.; Boccia, M.; Piccardi, L.; Cimadevilla, J.M. Differences in spatial memory recognition due to cognitive style. Front. Pharmacol. 2017, 8, 550. [CrossRef] 47. Niaz, M.; De Nunez, G.S. The relationship of mobility-fixity to creativity formal reasoning and intelligence. J. Creat. Behav. 1991, 25, 205–217. [CrossRef] 48. Teghil, A.; Boccia, M.; Guariglia, C. Field dependence–independence differently affects retrospective time estimation and flicker-induced time dilation. Exp. Brain Res. 2019, 237, 1019–1029. [CrossRef] 49. Mefoh, P.C.; Nwoke, M.B.; Chukwuorji, J.C.; Chijioke, A.O. Effect of cognitive style and gender on adolescents’ problem solving ability. Think. Ski. Creat. 2017, 25, 47–52. [CrossRef] 50. Palmiero, M.; Nakatani, C.; Raver, D.; Belardinelli, M.O.; van Leeuwen, C. Abilities within and across visual and verbal domains: How specific is their influence on creativity? Creat. Res. J. 2010, 22, 369–377. [CrossRef] 51. Palmiero, M.; Cardi, V.; Belardinelli, M.O. The role of vividness of visual mental imagery on different dimensions of creativity. Creat. Res. J. 2011, 23, 372–375. [CrossRef] 52. Palmiero, M.; Nori, R.; Piccardi, L. Visualizer cognitive style enhances visual creativity. Neurosci. Lett. 2016, 615, 98–101. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 53. Sack, A.T.; Jacobs, C.; De Martino, F.; Staeren, N.; Goebel, R.; Formisano, E. Dynamic premotor-to-parietal interactions during spatial imagery. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 8417–8429. [CrossRef] 54. Roskos-ewoldsen, B.; Black, S.R.; McCown, S.M. Age-related changes in creative thinking. J. Creat. Behav. 2008, 42, 33–59. [CrossRef] 55. Finke, R.A.; Pinker, S.; Farah, M.J. Reinterpreting visual patterns in mental imagery. Cogn. Sci. 1989, 13, 51–78. [CrossRef] 56. Palmiero, M.; Piccardi, L. Is Visual Creativity Embodied? Thinking Aloud while Performing the Creative Mental Synthesis Task. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 455. [CrossRef] 57. Hindal, H.; Reid, N.; Badgaish, M. Working memory, performance and learner characteristics. Res. Sci. Technol. 2009, 27, 187–204. [CrossRef] Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 212 12 of 12 58. Evans, C.; Richardson, J.T.; Waring, M. Field independence: Reviewing the evidence. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 83, 210–224. [CrossRef] 59. Boccia, M.; Piccardi, L.; Di Marco, M.; Pizzamiglio, L.; Guariglia, C. Does field independence predict visuo-spatial abilities underpinning human navigation? Behavioural evidence. Exp. Brain Res. 2016, 234, 2799–2807. [CrossRef] 60. Li, H.; Zhang, Y.; Wu, C.; Mei, D. Effects of field dependence-independence and frame of reference on navigation performance using multi-dimensional elec-tronic maps. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2016, 97, 289–299. [CrossRef] 61. Taylor, C.L.; Kaufman, J.C.; Barbot, B. Measuring creative writing with the storyboard task: The role of effort and story length. J. Creat. Behav. 2021, 55, 476–488. [CrossRef] 62. Burleson, B.R.; Samter, W.; Waltman, M.S. More evidence that cognitive complexity is not loquacity: A reply to beatty and payne. Commun. Q. 1987, 35, 317–328. [CrossRef]

Journal

Behavioral SciencesMultidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute

Published: Jun 25, 2022

Keywords: field dependent-independent cognitive styles; creativity; creative production; Geneplore model; visual creative synthesis task

There are no references for this article.