Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Parental Socialization and Its Impact across the Lifespan

Parental Socialization and Its Impact across the Lifespan behavioral sciences Article Parental Socialization and Its Impact across the Lifespan 1 1 2 , Jose Antonio Martinez-Escudero , Sonia Villarejo , Oscar F. Garcia * and Fernando Garcia Department of Methodology of Behavioral Sciences, Faculty of Psychology, University of Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain; jomares2@alumni.uv.es (J.A.M.-E.); sovite@alumni.uv.es (S.V.); fernando.garcia@uv.es (F.G.) Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain * Correspondence: oscar.f.garcia@uv.es Received: 16 April 2020; Accepted: 12 June 2020; Published: 16 June 2020 Abstract: Classical studies have found that parental warmth combined with parental strictness is the best parental strategy to promote children’s psychosocial development. Nevertheless, a growing set of emergent studies has questioned the benefits of parental strictness. The present study examined parental socialization and its short- and long-term impact on the psychosocial development of adolescents and adult children. The sample consisted of 2150 Spanish participants, 623 adolescents (12–18 years), 619 young adults (19–35 years), 502 middle-aged adults (35–59 years), and 406 older adults (60 years or older). Families were classified into one of four typologies (indulgent, authoritative, authoritarian, and neglectful). Psychosocial development was examined with five indicators (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). The results show a common short- and long-term pattern between parenting styles and psychosocial development: the indulgent style equaled or even surpassed the authoritative style, whereas the neglectful and authoritarian styles were associated with low scores. The present findings were discussed by considering the importance of the cultural context in family socialization. Additionally, the long-term impact of parental socialization seems to be crucial, even in adulthood. Keywords: parental socialization; adolescence; adult development 1. Introduction Parental socialization consists of parents’ influence on their children, in order to, among other objectives, encourage them to learn to inhibit actions that may be annoying or harmful to others and, at the same time, acquire behaviors that society demands, including consideration for others, self-reliance, accepting responsibility, and skills that will help them to become competent adults [1]. As socialization theorists explain, modern societies cannot rely on the ubiquitous, permanent, and indefinite presence of police or supervisors (for example, parents or caregivers) to ensure that individuals conform to social norms [2,3]. For this reason, the parents’ own socializing actions must lead the child to a certain degree of self-regulation (i.e., self-control) with regard to social norms. There comes a time when parental socialization ends: the teenager is now an adult. Unfortunately, not all children reach the socialization goals and become responsible adult members of their society [4,5]. In the study of parental socialization, Diana Baumrind defined a tripartite model based on the interaction between a ection, communication, and control, yielding three parenting styles: authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive [6]; these three parenting patterns corresponded to three modes of parental control, the authoritative control, the authoritarian control, and the lack of control (i.e., Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101; doi:10.3390/bs10060101 www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 2 of 17 Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 2 of 18 modes of parental control, the authoritative control, the authoritarian control, and the lack of control permissive control) [7,8] (see Figure 1). However, the theoretical framework proposed by Maccoby (i.e., permissive control) [7,8] (see Figure 1). However, the theoretical framework proposed by and Martin (1983) [1] became the referential model for the study of parental socialization using Maccoby and Martin (1983) [1] became the referential model for the study of parental socialization two theoretically orthogonal axes, warmth (also called responsiveness, acceptance, or a ection) and using two theoretically orthogonal axes, warmth (also called responsiveness, acceptance, or affection) strictness (also called demandingness or imposition), and four styles [9–13]. and strictness (also called demandingness or imposition), and four styles [9–13]. 1.0 Strictness 1.0 Strictness 0.8 0.8 Psychological control Behavioral control 0.6 0.6 Authoritarian Authoritative Authoritarian Authoritative 0.4 0.4 0.2 0.2 Warmth Warmth 0.0 0.0 -0.2 -0.2 -0.4 -0.4 -0.6 Permissive -0.6 Neglectful Indulgent -0.8 -0.8 -1.0 -1.0 -1.0 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 -1.0 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 (a) (b) Figure 1. (a) Theoretical tripartite model ‘Y’; Representation of the tripartite model ‘Y’ proposed Figure 1. (a) Theoretical tripartite model ‘Y’; Representation of the tripartite model ‘Y’ proposed by Diana Baumrind [7]. (b) Theoretical fourfold model based on the two orthogonal dimensions; by Diana Baumrind [7]. (b) Theoretical fourfold model based on the two orthogonal Representation of the fourfold model in which parenting practices and styles are examined within the dimensions; Representation of the fourfold model in which parenting practices and styles are two orthogonal dimensions [1,11,14–16]. examined within the two orthogonal dimensions [1,11,14–16]. It is important to note that scholars for these two orthogonal parental dimensions used di erent It is important to note that scholars for these two orthogonal parental dimensions used different labels, labe although ls, althoutheir gh their operationalization operationalizationwas was ver very y s similar imilar. Hi . Historically storically, th , e w theawarmth rmth dimens dimension ion has has been labeled by early scholars as acceptance [17], assurance [18], nurturance [19,20], warmth [21,22], been labeled by early scholars as acceptance [17], assurance [18], nurturance [19,20], warmth [21,22], or love [14]; all of these labels have a similar meanings to acceptance/involvement [15]. The strictness or love [14]; all of these labels have a similar meanings to acceptance/involvement [15]. The strictness dimensions has also been labeled as domination [17], hostility [18], inflexibility [22], control [14,23], dimensions has also been labeled as domination [17], hostility [18], inflexibility [22], control [14,23], firm firm control [24] or restriction [21]; all of these labels have a similar meanings to strictness/supervision control [24] or restriction [21]; all of these labels have a similar meanings to strictness/supervision [15]. [15]. In this sense, although schoolars have worked from different theoretical perspectives (e.g., In this sense, although schoolars have worked from di erent theoretical perspectives (e.g., behaviorist behaviorist and Freudian theory), they described a simmilar parenting patterns [3,11,25]. As and Freudian theory), they described a simmilar parenting patterns [3,11,25]. As Steinberg noted Steinberg noted (2005, p. 71), “responsiveness was often operationalized using measures of parental (2005, p. 71), “responsiveness was often operationalized using measures of parental warmth and warmth and acceptance, while demandingness came to be defined with respect to parental firmness" acceptance, while demandingness came to be defined with respect to parental firmness” [24]. The four [24]. The four parenting styles are defined by combining the two orthogonal dimensions: parenting authorit styles ative parents are defined (high warmth and high strictne by combining the two orthogonal ss), authorit dimensions: arian parentauthoritative s (low warmth an par d hi ents gh(high strictness), indulgent parents (high warmth and low strictness), and neglectful parents (low warmth warmth and high strictness), authoritarian parents (low warmth and high strictness), indulgent parents and low strictness) [26–28]. The empirical validation of the Maccoby and Martin model (1983) [1] with (high warmth and low strictness), and neglectful parents (low warmth and low strictness) [26–28]. a sample of more than 10,000 adolescents and their families [15] showed the need to distinguish The empirical validation of the Maccoby and Martin model (1983) [1] with a sample of more than between two types of "permissive" parents: indulgent (with warmth) and neglectful (without 10,000 adolescents and their families [15] showed the need to distinguish between two types of warmth). “permissive” parents: indulgent (with warmth) and neglectful (without warmth). The fourfold theoretical parenting model (i.e., authoritative, indulgent, authoritarian and The fourfold theoretical parenting model (i.e., authoritative, indulgent, authoritarian and neglectful) emphasizes the need to examine the combined effects of the two parental dimensions (i.e., neglectful) emphasizes the need to examine the combined e ects of the two parental dimensions (i.e., warmth and strictness) in order to accurately analyze the relationship between parental socialization warmth and strictness) in order to accurately analyze the relationship between parental socialization and child adjustment [15]. The two parental dimensions would express two types of persistent patterns in parental strategies, theoretically defined as independent or orthogonal constructs (they and child adjustment [15]. The two parental dimensions would express two types of persistent patterns are not related, and parenting in one of them does not allow us to know what parenting will be like in parental strategies, theoretically defined as independent or orthogonal constructs (they are not in the other). Furthermore, the fourfold theoretical parenting model also helps researchers to organize related, and parenting in one of them does not allow us to know what parenting will be like in the other). Furthermore, the fourfold theoretical parenting model also helps researchers to organize di erent parenting practices according to these parental dimensions [1,11] (see Figure 1). For example, parenting practices based on psychological control are characterized by high strictness and low warmth, which are Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 3 of 17 Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 3 of 18 related diff toerent pa authoritarian renting pra parents ctices a [29]; ccordi parenting ng to these practices parental dim of behavioral ensions contr [1, ol 11] ( arescharacterized ee Figure 1). F by or high example, parenting practices based on psychological control are characterized by high strictness and strictness and high warmth, which are related to authoritative parents [30]. Parenting practices such as low warmth, which are related to authoritarian parents [29]; parenting practices of behavioral control reasoning and dialogue to limit the child’s incorrect behaviors are positively related to the parental are characterized by high strictness and high warmth, which are related to authoritative parents [30]. warmth dimension, which is related to authoritative and indulgent parents [31]. Di erent empirical Parenting practices such as reasoning and dialogue to limit the child’s incorrect behaviors are studies have tested the organization of di erent parental practices on the two main parental dimensions positively related to the parental warmth dimension, which is related to authoritative and indulgent across di erent cultural contexts as United States [30] or Europe [32,33] (see Figure 2). In general, parents [31]. Different empirical studies have tested the organization of different parental practices the main findings from these studies agreed on the organization of di erent parental practices on on the two main parental dimensions across different cultural contexts as United States [30] or Europe the two main parental dimensions. Furthermore, the analysis of the impact of parenting practices [32,33] (see Figure 2). In general, the main findings from these studies agreed on the organization of on child different p adjustment arental is pra qui cti te ces on the two accurate in examining main parent them al dim within ensions the . Furthermor general context e, the anof alysi the s of par the enting impact of parenting practices on child adjustment is quite accurate in examining them within the style (see Darling and Steinberg, 1993) [11]. For example, monitoring was originally identified as general context of the parenting style (see Darling and Steinberg, 1993) [11]. For example, monitoring a parenting practice related to parental e orts to watch over their children as a form of strict or was originally identified as a parenting practice related to parental efforts to watch over their children firm control [34,35]. Overall, parental monitoring predicted a wide range of adjustment outcomes. as a form of strict or firm control [34,35]. Overall, parental monitoring predicted a wide range of Nevertheless, most of the relationships between monitoring and adjustment outcomes can be explained adjustment outcomes. Nevertheless, most of the relationships between monitoring and adjustment by children’s spontaneous disclosure of information to their parents (authoritative parenting), but not outcomes can be explained by children's spontaneous disclosure of information to their parents by parents’ tracking and surveillance e orts (authoritarian parenting) (see Stattin and Kerr, 2000; Kerr (authoritative parenting), but not by parents' tracking and surveillance efforts (authoritarian and Stattin, 2000) [36,37]. parenting) (see Stattin and Kerr, 2000; Kerr and Stattin, 2000) [36,37]. 1.0 1.0 Protection [s- EMBU] Strictness 0.8 Strictness 0.8 Psychological control Behavioral Psychological control Control APM 0.6 0.6 Authoritarian Authoritative Authoritarian Authoritative Rejection [s-EMBU] 0.4 0.4 0.2 0.2 Emotional Warmth [s- EMBU] Rejection 0.0 0.0 Warmth Support and Warmth communication Acceptance/ -0.2 Involvement [APM] -0.2 -0.4 -0.4 -0.6 Neglectful Indulgent -0.6 Neglectful Indulgent -0.8 -0.8 Neglect Psychological Autonomy Granting [APM] -1.0 -1.0 -1.0 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 -1.0 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 (a) (b) Figure 2. Representation of the fourfold model in which parenting practices and styles are examined Figure 2. Representation of the fourfold model in which parenting practices and styles are examined within the two orthogonal dimensions in the Spanish cultural context. (a) Garcia et al., 2015; Empirical within the two orthogonal dimensions in the Spanish cultural context. (a) Garcia et al., 2015; Empirical study of Garcia et al., 2015 [32]. (b) Tur-Porcar et al., 2019; Empirical study of Tur-Porcar et al., study of Garcia et al., 2015 [32]. (b) Tur-Porcar et al., 2019; Empirical study of Tur-Porcar et al., 2019 [APM] [BAR] [APM] [BAR] 2019 [33]. Note. The Authoritative Parenting Measure, APM [15,38]. The Psychological [33]. Note. The Authoritative Parenting Measure, APM [15,38]. The Psychological Control [s-EMBU [s-EMBU] ] ContrScale – Y ol Scale–Y outh Self-Report, outh Self-Report, PCS-YSR [39]. PCS-YSR [39]. The S(ho The rt)-EMBU [40– S(hort)-EMBU 42]. The measures of Tur- [40–42]. The measures of Porcar et al. 2019 are from Child Reports of Parental Behavior Inventory, CRPBI [43,44]. Tur-Porcar et al. 2019 are from Child Reports of Parental Behavior Inventory, CRPBI [43,44]. Empirical research on parental socialization, mainly in Anglo-Saxon cultural contexts with Empirical research on parental socialization, mainly in Anglo-Saxon cultural contexts with European-American middle-class families, indicates that authoritative parents produce children with European-American middle-class families, indicates that authoritative parents produce children better psychosocial development, compared to children from the other three types of families with better psychosocial development, compared to children from the other three types of [3,6,11,15]. However, results of studies with ethnic minorities from the United States, for example, families [3,6,11,15]. However, results of studies with ethnic minorities from the United States, for Chinese-Americans [45,46], Hispanic-Americans [47,48], or African-Americans [49,50], as well as example, Chinese-Americans [45,46], Hispanic-Americans [47,48], or African-Americans [49,50], as studies in Arab societies [51,52], question the idea that authoritative parenting (parental strictness well as studies in Arab societies [51,52], question the idea that authoritative parenting (parental with warmth) is always the most appropriate parental socialization style. These studies show that the strictness with warmth) is always the most appropriate parental socialization style. These studies show that the authoritarian style (parental strictness without warmth) is positively related to some indicators of psychosocial development in children and adolescents. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 4 of 17 Likewise, a growing set of studies, carried out mainly with families from European [53,54] and Latin-American countries [55,56], suggests that the indulgent style (parental warmth without strictness) is associated with equal or even greater psychosocial development than the authoritative style [57–60]. Children with indulgent parents, compared to their peers in authoritative families, show equal or even better adjustment and competence indices on indicators of optimal psychosocial development, such as self-concept [53,54], emotional regulation [61], school competence [62], internalization of values [63], including self-transcendence (universalism and benevolence) and conservation (security, conformity, and tradition), or connectivity with the environment [64], and protection from problems such as substance use [65], alcohol consumption [66], personal maladjustment indicators [67], and emotional maladjustment, including nervousness and hostility [58], sexist attitudes [68], and cyber-aggression and cybervictimization [69]. The parental socialization literature shows discrepant results about the relationship between parenting styles and child development, depending on the ethnic, socioeconomic, or cultural context where parental socialization takes place [9,70–73]. Additionally, although the impact of parental socialization is known to be crucial for the child, once adolescence is over and it is necessary to have the skills demanded by the adult world [1,74,75], few studies have examined long-term parental socialization beyond adolescence. Most of these studies have been limited to young adults [58], or adolescents and older adults have been compared using di erent adjustment criteria [76]. Although developmental theorists have highlighted the importance of early socialization experiences on development well beyond adolescence [3,77], less is known about the links between parenting and developmental outcomes in adulthood [76]. This may be due to the many non-normative influences in adulthood or the time elapsed since the early socialization experiences [78]. Therefore, a crucial question is whether di erences in development, in both adolescent and adult children, can be consistently related to parenting styles. In the present study, the relationship between parental socialization styles (i.e., indulgent, authoritative, authoritarian, and neglectful) and short-term and long-term psychosocial development in adolescents and adults (young, middle-aged, and older) was examined through a wide range of indicators (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Importantly, some previous parenting studies have examined these indicators of psychosocial development, but in isolation rather than simultaneously. Overall, these indicators have been identified as important for psychosocial development. Physical self-concept, which includes self-perceptions about physical appearance and physical performance, is positively related to physical fitness [79] and a protective factor against eating disorders [80] or food neophobia [81]. Family self-concept, which includes self-perceptions about feeling loved and appreciated by the family, is positively related to general self-esteem [82] or secure attachment [83]. Nervousness is an indicator of emotional instability that is linked to aggressive behaviors or alcohol consumption [84,85]. Empathy, which involves sharing someone else’s perceived emotion—“feeling with” another [86], is related to more prosocial behaviors and less aggressive behaviors [87]. The values of benevolence are part of the self-transcendence values, identified in the Swartz theoretical framework as being strongly connected to prosocial behaviors and empathy [88,89]. In particular, benevolence values might be more connected to empathy than the other nine social values proposed by Swartz [90]. Based on previous studies, children from indulgent families are expected to show equal or even better psychosocial development on all the indicators than children from authoritative families, whereas children from neglectful and authoritarian families are expected to obtain the worst adjustment on all the indicators. 2. Materials and Methods 2.1. Participants and Procedure The present study involved 2150 adolescent and adult children (M = 35.63, SD = 20.67), 1262 women (57.7%) and 888 men (58.7%): Adolescents (n = 623, 361 women, 59.7%) from 12 to 18 years old (M = 16.57; SD = 1.72); young adults (n = 619, 366 women, 59.1%) from 19 to 35 years old (M = 23.68 SD = 3.87); Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 5 of 17 middle-aged adults (n = 502, 322 women, 64.1%) from 36 to 59 years old (M = 48.30; SD = 6.32); and older adults (n = 406, 213 women, 52.5%) over 60 years old (M = 68.66: SD = 7.80). Applying an a priori power analysis [91,92], the minimum sample required was 2150 observations to detect a power of 0.89 ( = 0.05; 1 = 0.95), with a medium-low e ect size, f = 0.17, estimated value with the ANOVAs on a univariate F test of the four parenting styles [15,93]. Following previous studies on socialization with adolescent children and adult children, a similar procedure was followed for data collection in this study [5,58,63]. In short, adolescents were recruited from high schools that were selected randomly from the complete list of high schools; young adults from university undergraduate courses; middle aged-adults from city council neighborhoods; and older adults from senior citizen centers that were selected randomly from the complete list of senior citizen centers. Data collection was conducted during the 2017–2018 and 2018–2019 academic years. All the participants in this study: (a) were Spanish, as were their parents and their four grandparents; (b) lived in nuclear families with two parents, a mother or female primary caregiver and a father or male primary caregiver; (c) participated voluntarily, both adolescents and adult children, with the prior consent of their parents to participate in the case of the adolescents; (d) filled out the self-report paper-and-pencil questionnaires. According to the Declaration of Helsinki, the protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Program for the Promotion of Scientific Research, Technological Development, and Innovation of the Spanish Valencian Region, the public institution that supports the present research. This study was also approved by the College Research Ethics Committee (CREC) of the Nottingham Trent University (NTU, Nottingham, UK; Project No. 2017/90). 2.2. Measures 2.2.1. Parental Socialization The parental warmth dimension was measured with the 20 items on the Warmth/A ection Scale, originally developed by Rohner (1978) [94] and widely used in parenting studies [95]. This scale o ers a reliable measure of the degree to which children experience involvement and a ection from their parents (Examples of items: “Say nice things to me when I deserve them” and “Are really interested in what I do”), and it also has an adult version (Examples of items: “Said nice things to me when I deserved it” and “Were really interested in what I did”). An alpha coecient of 0.945 was obtained. The parental strictness dimension was measured with the 13 items on the Parental Control Scale, originally developed by Rohner (1978) [94], and widely used in parenting studies [96]. This scale o ers a reliable measure of the degree to which children experience control with firmness, demand, and severity by their parents (Examples of items: “Give me certain jobs to do and will not let me do anything else until I am done” and “Want to control whatever I do”), and it also has an adult version (Examples of items: “Gave me certain jobs to do and would not let me do anything else until I was done” and “Wanted to control whatever I did”). An alpha coecient of 0.904 was obtained. Both parental measures use a Likert-type response scale that ranges from 1 “almost never true” to 4 “almost always true”. Higher scores on the two parental measures represent high parental warmth and strictness. The four parenting styles were defined following the dichotomization procedure based on the median of the scores on each of the two parental dimensions (i.e., warmth and strictness) and examining them simultaneously: Authoritative families scored above the median on both parental dimensions, neglectful families scored below the median on both parental dimensions, indulgent families scored above the median on parental warmth and below the median on parental strictness, and authoritarian families scored below the median on parental warmth and above the median on parental strictness [5,15,46]. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 6 of 17 2.2.2. Psychosocial Development Physical and family self-concept. These two dimensions of self-concept were measured with the family and physical scales of the AF5 Self-Concept Questionnaire [97]. The AF5 is a widely validated questionnaire in samples of adolescents and adults [98–100] in several countries, such as Spain [99], Portugal [100], Brazil [101], Chile [102], China [103], or the United States [104]. The AF5 theoretical factorial structure (i.e. multidimensional) is invariant across Western and non-Western societies [101,103,104]. The physical component refers to the individual’s perception of his/her condition and physical appearance (Example item: “I like the way I look). An alpha coecient of 0.782 was obtained. The family component refers to the individual’s perception of his/her integration, involvement, and participation in the family environment (Example item: “My family would help me with any type of problem”). An alpha coecient of 0.816 was obtained. Both measures of self-concept, with six items each, use a Likert-type response scale ranging from 1 “Strong disagreement” to 99 “Strong agreement”. Higher scores on both measures indicate a higher sense of family and physical self-concept. Nervousness. It was measured with the 8 items on the nervousness scale of the Psychosocial Maturity Questionnaire [5,58,105]. Nervousness refers to the lack of emotional stability and anxiety in situations in everyday life (Example item: “My mood changes easily”). The scale uses a Likert-type response format ranging from 1 “Strongly disagree” to 5 “Strongly agree”. Higher scores on this scale indicate a higher degree of nervousness. An alpha coecient of 0.778 was obtained. Empathy. It was measured with the 5 items on the empathy scale of the Psychosocial Maturity Questionnaire [5,58,105]. Empathy refers to understanding others and considering other views apart from one’s own (Example item: “I am sensitive to others’ feelings and needs”). An alpha coecient of 0.672 was obtained. The scale uses a Likert-type response format ranging from 1 “Strongly disagree” to 5 “Strongly agree”. High scores on this scale indicate a high degree of empathy. Internalization of social values. The benevolence values were measured with the 5 benevolence scale items on the Schwartz Value Inventory [89,106]. The values of benevolence refer to the care of family relationships and values such as forgiveness (Example item: “Forgiving (Willing to pardon others)”). An alpha coecient of 0.740 was obtained. The scale uses a Likert-type response format ranging from 1 “Opposed to my values” to 99 “of supreme importance”. Scores on this scale indicate that a high priority is given to benevolence values. 2.3. Plan of Analysis A MANOVA (multivariate factorial analysis of variance) (4  2  4) was performed on the five indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence), with parenting styles (authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful), sex (women vs. men), and age (adolescents, young adults, middle-aged adults, and older adults) as independent variables. Univariate F tests were applied to all the sources of variation where statistically significant di erences were found on multivariate tests. The significant univariate results were followed by post-hoc Bonferroni comparisons for all the possible pairs of averages [5,107,108]. 3. Results 3.1. Parenting Styles Participants were distributed in the four family types (see Table 1). On parental warmth, children from indulgent (M = 73.47; SD = 4.63) and authoritative (M = 72.45, SD = 4.37) families scored higher than children from authoritarian (M = 55.00; SD = 10.04) and neglectful (M = 57.04; SD = 9.52) families. On parental strictness, the children from authoritative (M = 40.05, SD = 5.00) and authoritarian (M = 41.90; SD = 5.52) families scored higher than the children from indulgent (M = 28.35, SD = 5.57) and neglectful (M = 28.25, SD = 5.79). The results from the correlation analysis confirmed the Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 7 of 17 orthogonal theoretical assumption; the correlation between the two parental dimensions (i.e., warmth and strictness) was modest, r = 0.268, R = 0.072, less than 8%, p < 0.001 (the range in the shared variance, i.e., R , is from 0 to 1). Table 1. Distribution of participants by parenting style. Total Indulgent Authoritative Authoritarian Neglectful Warmth Mean 64.48 73.47 72.45 55.00 57.04 SD 1.52 4.63 4.37 10.04 9.52 Strictness Mean 34.69 28.35 40.05 41.90 28.25 SD 8.46 5.57 5.00 5.52 5.79 N (%) 2150 (100%) 628 (29.20%) 451 (21.00) 620 (28.80%) 451 (21.00%) 3.2. MANOVAs Multivariate Analysis Statistically significant di erences were obtained for the main e ects of parenting style, L = 0.742, F(15, 5836.2) = 44.45, p < 0.001; sex, L = 0.881, F(5, 2114.0) = 57.34, p < 0.001; and age, L = 0.895, F(15, 5836.2), = 16.02, p < 0.001 (see Table 2). The interaction e ects between parenting styles and sex also reached the level of statistical significance, L = 0.985, F(15, 5836.2) = 2.17, p = 0.006; as did the e ects between parenting styles and age, L = 0.958, F(45, 10562.0) = 2.00, p < 0.001; and between sex and age, L = 0.987, F(15, 5836.2) = 1.87, p = 0.021. a b c Table 2. Multivariate factorial analysis of variance (4  2  3 ) for the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values). Source of Variation L F df df p between error (A) Parenting Style 0.742 44.45 15 5836.2 <0.001 0.881 57.34 5 2114.0 <0.001 (B) Sex (C) Age 0.895 16.02 15 5836.2 <0.001 A  B 0.985 2.17 15 5836.2 0.006 A  C 0.958 2.00 45 9459.5 <0.001 B  C 0.987 1.87 15 5836.2 0.021 A  B  C 0.974 1.23 45 9459.5 0.138 a b c a , indulgent, a , authoritative, a , authoritarian, a , neglectful; b , male, b , female; c , adolescents, c , 1 2 3 4 1 2 1 2 young adults, c , middle-aged adults, c , older adults. 3 4 3.3. Parenting Styles On the psychosocial adjustment criteria, the indulgent style was related to equal or even better scores than the authoritative style, whereas the lowest scores corresponded to the neglectful and authoritarian styles (see Table 3). (i) On physical self-concept, an interaction effect between parenting style and sex was found, F(3, 2118) = 2.82, p = 0.037 (see Figure 3). Although women always scored lower than men, a similar pattern was seen when examining parenting styles. In men, indulgent and authoritative styles were related to better physical self-concept than neglectful and authoritarian styles. In women, indulgent and authoritative styles were associated with the highest scores, whereas the authoritarian style was associated with the lowest scores. (ii) On family self-concept, an interaction effect between parenting style and age was found, F(9, 2118) = 5.49, p < 0.001 (see Figure 1). The indulgent style was related to higher scores than the authoritative style, a trend that was observed within the groups of teenagers and young adults. The authoritative style was associated with higher scores than the authoritarian and neglectful styles. Finally, the lowest scores on family self-concept were related to the authoritarian style, a trend that was observed in the groups of teenagers and young adults, but not in middle-aged adults, whereas in the group of older adults, the lowest scores corresponded to the neglectful style. (iii) In the case of nervousness, the children from indulgent and authoritative families obtained lower scores than Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 8 of 17 their peers from authoritarian and neglectful households. (iv) For empathy, an interaction effect between parenting style and age was found, F(9, 2118) = 2.80, p = 0.003 (see Figure 1). Among the adolescents, the indulgent style was related to the highest scores on empathy, and the lowest scores corresponded to the authoritarian and neglectful parenting styles, with the authoritative style in an intermediate position. Similarly, in adult children, the indulgent and authoritative styles were associated with the highest scores (in all three groups), whereas the lowest scores corresponded to the authoritarian (in young adults) and neglectful (in young, middle-aged, and older adults) styles. (v) On the internalization of social values of benevolence, children with indulgent and authoritative parents reported higher scores than children from authoritarian and neglectful families. Table 3. Means, standard deviations (in parenthesis), F values, and Bonferroni’s test across parenting styles on the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Psychosocial Indulgent Authoritative Authoritarian Neglectful F(1, 2118) p Development 1 1 2 2 Physical self-concept 5.94 6.15 5.39 5.61 18.10 <0.001 (1.74) (1.79) (1.83) (1.68) 1 2 4 3 Family self-concept 8.74 8.51 7.13 7.41 212.01 <0.001 (0.95) (1.04) (1.57) (1.50) 2 2 1 1 Nervousness 2.20 2.29 2.56 2.49 38.06 <0.001 (0.63) (0.61) (0.64) (0.63) 1 1 2 2 Empathy 55.32 <0.001 4.11 4.05 3.81 3.74 (0.50) (0.52) (0.61) (0.63) 1 1 2 2 Social values 8.26 8.20 7.80 7.67 40.33 <0.001 of benevolence (1.04) (1.03) (1.20) (1.21) Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 9 of 18 Note: Bonferroni test = 0.05; 1 > 2 > 3 > 4. 7.5 9.5 Sex Parenting styles 9.04 9.0 7.0 8.90 Indulgent Women Authoritative 6.71 8.58 8.5 8.51 8.74 6.5 8.26 Authoritarian 6.28 Men 8.43 8.24 Neglectful 8.0 6.0 5.86 5.66 5.86 7.75 7.70 5.72 7.5 5.5 7.27 7.18 5.44 7.14 7.0 7.11 7.00 5.07 5.0 6.74 6.5 4.5 Adole s ce nts Young adults M iddle -age d Olde r adults Indulge nt Authoritative Authortiarian Ne ge lctful adults Parenting styles Age (a) (b) Parenting styles 4.16 4.2 4.10 4.12 4.1 Indulgent 4.12 4.03 4.09 Authoritative 4.0 4.02 3.94 3.90 Authoritarian 3.92 3.9 Neglectful 3.87 3.8 3.77 3.77 3.7 3.70 3.70 3.6 3.5 3.50 3.4 Adole s ce nts Young adults Middle -age d Olde r adults adults Age (c) Figure 3. Interaction e ects between parenting style and sex. (a) Physical self-concept. Interaction e ects Figure 3. Interaction effects between parenting style and sex. (a) Physical self-concept. Interaction between par effects between enting style parenting styl and age. ( e and age. ( b) Family b)self-concept, Family self-conc and ept, and (c) ( Empathy c) Empath. y. 3.4. Sex-and-Age-Related Differences Statistically significant sex differences were found on the five psychosocial development criteria examined (see Table 4). On physical self-concept, men scored higher than women. On family self- concept, an interaction between sex and age was found, F(3, 2118) = 3.79, p = 0.010 (see Figure 4). Women indicated a higher family self-concept than men, a trend that was observed in adolescents and young adults and, to a lesser extent, in middle-aged adults (in older adults, men and women scored the same). On nervousness, empathy, and social values of benevolence, women scored higher than men. Table 4. Means, standard deviations (in parenthesis), F values, and Bonferroni’s test between women and men on the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Psychosocial Development Women Men F(1, 2118) p Physical self-concept 5.46 6.18 83.57 < 0.001 (1.76) (1.75) Family self-concept 8.07 7.76 21.11 < 0.001 (1.44) (1.51) Nervousness 4.04 3.78 104.42 < 0.001 (0.57) (0.58) Empathy 2.44 2.31 22.13 < 0.001 Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 9 of 17 3.4. Sex-and-Age-Related Di erences Statistically significant sex di erences were found on the five psychosocial development criteria examined (see Table 4). On physical self-concept, men scored higher than women. On family self-concept, an interaction between sex and age was found, F(3, 2118) = 3.79, p = 0.010 (see Figure 4). Women indicated a higher family self-concept than men, a trend that was observed in adolescents and young adults and, to a lesser extent, in middle-aged adults (in older adults, men and women scored the same). On nervousness, empathy, and social values of benevolence, women scored higher than men. Table 4. Means, standard deviations (in parenthesis), F values, and Bonferroni’s test between women and men on the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Psychosocial Women Men F(1, 2118) p Development Physical 5.46 6.18 83.57 <0.001 self-concept (1.76) (1.75) Family self-concept 8.07 7.76 21.11 <0.001 (1.44) (1.51) Nervousness 4.04 3.78 104.42 <0.001 Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 10 of 18 (0.57) (0.58) Empathy 2.44 2.31 22.13 <0.001 (0.66) (0.62) (0.66) (0.62) Social values 8.13 7.79 42.94 < 0.001 Social values 8.13 7.79 42.94 <0.001 of benevolence (1.12) (1.16) of benevolence (1.12) (1.16) Note: Bonferroni test α = 0.05. Note: Bonferroni test = 0.05. 8.5 Sex 8.4 8.27 8.3 Women 8.24 8.2 8.1 Men 8.0 7.93 7.92 7.9 7.8 7.74 7.73 7.7 7.67 7.6 7.62 7.5 Adole sce nts Young adults M iddle-age d Olde r adults adults Age Figure 4. Interaction between sex and age. Family self-concept. Figure 4. Interaction between sex and age. Family self-concept. Statistically significant di erences in age were found on the five psychosocial development criteria examined (see Table 5). On physical self-concept, adolescents and young adults obtained higher Statistically significant differences in age were found on the five psychosocial development scores than middle-aged adults, who, in turn, scored higher than older adults. On family self-concept, criteria examined (see Table 5). On physical self-concept, adolescents and young adults obtained examining the sex profile by age (see Figure 4), the highest scores in women corresponded to the age higher scores than middle-aged adults, who, in turn, scored higher than older adults. On family self- group of adolescents and young adults, whereas the men who scored the highest were the young adults. concept, examining the sex profile by age (see Figure 4), the highest scores in women corresponded Onnervousness, adolescents and young adults scored higher than middle-aged and older adults. to the age group of adolescents and young adults, whereas the men who scored the highest were the On empathy, young adults and middle-aged adults reported higher scores than adolescents and young adults. On nervousness, adolescents and young adults scored higher than middle-aged and older adults. On priority given to benevolence values, middle-aged adults and older adults obtained older adults. On empathy, young adults and middle-aged adults reported higher scores than the highest scores, whereas the lowest scores corresponded to adolescents (older adults exceeded adolescents and older adults. On priority given to benevolence values, middle-aged adults and older young adults). adults obtained the highest scores, whereas the lowest scores corresponded to adolescents (older adults exceeded young adults). Table 5. Means, standard deviations (in parenthesis), F values, and Bonferroni’s test across age groups on the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Psychosocial Young Middle-aged Older Adolescents F(3, 2118) p Development Adults Adults Adults 1 1 2 3 Physical self-concept 6.13 5.96 5.54 5.15 33.72 < 0.001 (1.83) (1.78) (1.71) (1.65) a 1 2 2. b Family self-concept 8.03 8.13 7.86 7.65 13.83 < 0.001 (1.49) (1.46) (1.43) (1.48) 1 1 2 2 Nervousness 2.44 2.40 2.33 2.34 2.69 < 0.001 (0.64) (0.65) (0.65) (0.64) 2.b 1 1. a 2 Empathy 3.89 4.01 3.96 3.84 8.10 < 0.001 (0.57) (0.55) (0.58) (0.64) 2 b 1 1. a Social values 7.78 8.00 8.19 8.04 12.19 < 0.001 of benevolence (1.23) (1.09) (1.05) (1.18) Note: Bonferroni test α = .05; 1 > 2 > 3; a > b. 4. Discussion This paper examined the relationship between parenting styles (indulgent, authoritative, authoritarian, and neglectful) and psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of benevolence social values) in the short term in teenagers, and in the long term in adult children (young adults, middle-aged adults, and older Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 10 of 17 Table 5. Means, standard deviations (in parenthesis), F values, and Bonferroni’s test across age groups on the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Psychosocial Young Middle-aged Older Adolescents F(3, 2118) p Development Adults Adults Adults Physical 1 1 2 3 6.13 5.96 5.54 5.15 33.72 <0.001 self-concept (1.83) (1.78) (1.71) (1.65) Family a 1 2 2,b 8.03 8.13 7.86 7.65 13.83 <0.001 self-concept (1.49) (1.46) (1.43) (1.48) 1 1 2 2 Nervousness 2.44 2.40 2.33 2.34 2.69 <0.001 (0.64) (0.65) (0.65) (0.64) 2,b 1 1,a 2 Empathy 3.89 4.01 3.96 3.84 8.10 <0.001 (0.57) (0.55) (0.58) (0.64) 2 b 1 1,a Social values 12.19 <0.001 7.78 8.00 8.19 8.04 of benevolence (1.23) (1.09) (1.05) (1.18) Note: Bonferroni test = 0.05; 1 > 2 > 3; a > b. 4. Discussion This paper examined the relationship between parenting styles (indulgent, authoritative, authoritarian, and neglectful) and psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of benevolence social values) in the short term in teenagers, and in the long term in adult children (young adults, middle-aged adults, and older adults). The measures of the parental dimensions (i.e., warmth and strictness) were modestly correlated, confirming the orthogonality theoretical assumption. The results of this study show a common pattern in the short and long term in the relationship between parenting styles and psychosocial development. The indulgent style (parental warmth without strictness) appears to be the optimal style, associated with equal or even better results than authoritative parenting (parental warmth with strictness), whereas the styles with a lack of parental warmth (authoritarian and neglectful) are related to low scores on the psychosocial development criteria examined. In general, the results of the present study indicate that the indulgent and authoritative styles were related to better psychosocial adjustment indicators than the authoritarian and neglectful styles. In the case of physical self-concept, examining the profile of parenting styles by sex, we observed that, although men showed a higher physical self-concept than women, parenting styles characterized by warmth (indulgent and authoritative) were related to a higher physical self-concept in both men and women. With regard to family self-concept, examining the profile of parenting styles by age, we observed that the indulgent style was related to a higher family self-concept than the authoritative style (in adolescents and young adults), and the authoritative style was associated with a higher family self-concept than the authoritarian style. Regarding the parenting styles without parental warmth, children raised in authoritarian families obtained the lowest family self-concept scores (although this trend was only observed in adolescents and young adults). In the case of empathy, the profile of parenting styles by age revealed that the indulgent style was related to equal or better results (in adolescents), whereas neglectful and authoritarian parenting styles were related to less empathy (in middle-aged adult children and older adults, those who were raised by neglectful families indicated the lowest empathy). In relation to benevolence, parenting styles characterized by warmth (indulgent and authoritative) gave a higher priority to this social value than parenting styles characterized by lack of warmth (neglectful and authoritarian). An important question that has been widely examined in the parenting literature for years has to do with the best parenting strategy to promote optimal psychosocial development [5,6,11]. The influence of parents on children is usually captured through parental warmth and strictness, both defined as orthogonal (i.e., unrelated) dimension [1,11]. The results from the present study confirm other previous Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 11 of 17 research from studies mainly conducted in European [53,54,57] and Latin-American countries [9,55,56], where indulgent parenting (i.e., warmth without strictness) is related to equal or even better results than authoritative parenting (i.e., warmth with strictness), thus extending the evidence about the benefits of indulgent parenting to five important indicators of psychosocial development. Nevertheless, the present findings also contradict the commonly accepted idea about the benefits of parental strictness combined with parental warmth for child development, based mainly on results from studies conducted with middle-class European-American families [6,11,15]. In these studies, children and adolescents from authoritative families have greater psychosocial development on di erent indicators than their peers from the other households [1,4,24]. It seems that children from families characterized by warmth and strictness (i.e., authoritative parenting) do not always develop the best competence and adjustment in all cultural contexts. For example, some previous research in ethnic minorities from the United States [46,49] as well as from Asian and Arab countries [51,52], revealed some benefits related to authoritarian parenting (i.e., warmth without strictness). Therefore, a particularly pressing issue is the di erent impact of the parenting styles depending on the child’s cultural background [11,71]. Another main contributions of this study is that it provides empirical evidence about the consistent links between parenting and psychosocial development, not only in the short term in adolescent children, but also in the long term, beyond adolescence, when parental socialization has ended, using five indicators of key psychosocial development in adolescence and adult life: the individual’s perception of him/herself in the physical and family environment (physical and family self-concept), the individual’s emotional stability (captured with an indicator of mismatch, that is, nervousness), the ability to understand and understand others (empathy), and the internalization of social values (priority given to benevolence). Theorists on parental socialization point out that during the socialization process, parents exert their influence on children through practices of a ection, dialogue, reasoning (parental warmth), and firm or restrictive practices (parental strictness), in order to achieve one of the main objectives of socialization: for the child to become a responsible adult [1–3]. The findings of the present study show that, in both adolescents and adults (young, middle-aged, and older), the di erences in psychosocial development can be theoretically predicted by the parental socialization style. Of the few previous studies that examined the long-term impact of parental socialization, most of them focused on young adults [58], they were conducted in countries with an Anglo-Saxon culture, or they compared adolescents and older adults using di erent adjustment criteria [76]. Therefore, the present study allows a more exhaustive comparison by using five identical key psychosocial criteria and examining the impact of short-term and long-term socialization in three groups of adults. In addition, although it is not the main objective of the study, our results coincide with sex and age di erences found in some previous studies [58,63,109]. Adolescence is frequently characterized as the developmental period in which the physical and social status of a child changes to that of an adult [110] (p. 112). Adolescents are not yet adults, and their parents act as socializing agents by using practices based on warmth and strictness. The present study showed important age-related di erences in the developmental indicators examined in adolescents and adults, but also among young, middle-aged, and older adults, confirming some results from developmental studies about age-related di erences across the life span [78,111]. In adolescence, but especially in adulthood, there are many influences (e.g., biological, personal, social and cultural) on development [57]. Despite these influences, at least in adolescence, previous parenting studies showed that di erences in competence and adjustment among adolescents can be consistently related to parenting styles—in the same cultural context—[11,71]. Much less is known about what happens in adulthood. Nevertheless, as in some previous emergent studies, the main findings revealed that di erences in competence and adjustment among adult children are also related to parenting. As in most of the previous studies with adult children [112,113], the present study follows a cross-sectional design, and so the results should be considered preliminary. Longitudinal evidence is more dicult to obtain for adult children, but the few longitudinal studies [114] have also revealed consistent links between parenting and adult development. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 12 of 17 This study has strengths and limitations. The use of the four-typology parental socialization model provides a common framework with which to compare the results of research conducted in di erent countries around the world. The impact of parental socialization is examined beyond adolescence, with three groups of adult children and five psychosocial development criteria. Therefore, in addition to contributing to the current debate on the best parental socialization strategy, the findings of this study allow us to verify whether di erences in psychosocial development between adults are also theoretically predictable based on parenting styles. As study limitations, it should be noted that the responses were provided by the adolescents and adult children [115], although there is evidence suggesting lower social desirability in children than in their parents [116]. Additionally, participants from the present study only include Spanish families and their adolescent and adult children. Therefore, future studies should extend these findings by examining parenting in immigrant families or ethnic minorities in Spanish society. In addition, the results of this study should be interpreted with caution, without inferring causal relationships or excluding possible third variables (especially considering the time elapsed for older adults), although these results are similar to those found in other studies with adolescents. 5. Conclusions This study makes a valuable contribution to the current international debate about the suitability of parental socialization styles [72,73,117]. On the one hand, the findings of this work coincide with a set of studies, mainly carried out in countries in Europe [53,54] and Latin America [55,56], suggesting that the indulgent style is the optimal parental socialization style. However, the findings of the present study do not coincide with the classic studies mainly carried out with samples of middle-class European-American families [3,6,11,15]. The cultural context where socialization occurs seems to be decisive in the relationship between parental socialization styles and the children’s pattern of competence and adjustment. Future studies should examine the most appropriate parental strategy to socialize children considering families from di erent ethnic and cultural contexts [118–121], as well as di erent individual characteristics of the children [122,123]. Author Contributions: Conceptualization, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; methodology, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G. and; software, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; validation, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; formal analysis, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; investigation, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; resources, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; data curation, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; writing—original draft preparation, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; writing—review and editing, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; visualization, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; supervision, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; project administration, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; funding acquisition, O.F.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript. Funding: This research was partially funded by grants ACIF/2016/431 and BEFPI/2017/058, which provided funding for a research stay at the Nottingham Trent University, UK (Valencian Regional Government, and European Social Fund), and FPU16/00988 (Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities, Government of Spain). Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest. References 1. Maccoby, E.E.; Martin, J.A. Socialization in the context of the family: Parent–child interaction. In Handbook of Child Psychology; Mussen, P.H., Ed.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1983; Volume 4, pp. 1–101. 2. Baumrind, D. Reciprocal Rights and Responsibilities in Parent-Child Relations. J. Soc. Issues 1978, 34, 179–196. [CrossRef] 3. Maccoby, E.E. The Role of Parents in the Socialization of Children—An Historical Overview. Dev. Psychol. 1992, 28, 1006–1017. [CrossRef] 4. Baumrind, D. Parental Disciplinary Patterns and Social Competence in Children. Youth Soc. 1978, 9, 239–276. [CrossRef] 5. Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E. Raising Children with Poor School Performance: Parenting Styles and Short- and Long-Term Consequences for Adolescent and Adult Development. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1089. [CrossRef] Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 13 of 17 6. Baumrind, D. Current patterns of Parental Authority. Dev. Psychol. 1971, 4, 1–103. [CrossRef] 7. Baumrind, D. E ects of Authoritative Parental Control on Child Behavior. Child Dev. 1966, 37, 887–907. [CrossRef] 8. Baumrind, D. Authoritarian Vs Authoritative Parental Control. Adolescence 1968, 3, 255–272. 9. Martinez, I.; Garcia, F.; Veiga, F.; Garcia, O.F.; Rodrigues, Y.; Serra, E. Parenting Styles, Internalization of Values and Self-Esteem: A Cross-Cultural Study in Spain, Portugal and Brazil. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2370. [CrossRef] 10. Martínez, I.; Cruise, E.; Garcia, O.F.; Murgui, S. English Validation of the Parental Socialization Scale—ESPA29. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1–10. [CrossRef] 11. Darling, N.; Steinberg, L. Parenting Style as Context: An Integrative Model. Psychol. Bull. 1993, 113, 487–496. [CrossRef] 12. Martínez, I.; Garcia, F.; Musitu, G.; Yubero, S. Family Socialization Practices: Factor Confirmation of the Portuguese Version of a Scale for their Measurement. Rev. Psicodidact. 2012, 17, 159–178. 13. Martinez, I.; Garcia, F.; Fuentes, M.C.; Veiga, F.; Garcia, O.F.; Rodrigues, Y.; Cruise, E.; Serra, E. Researching Parental Socialization Styles Across Three Cultural Contexts: Scale ESPA29 Bi-Dimensional Validity in Spain, Portugal, and Brazil. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 197. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 14. Schaefer, E.S. A Circumplex Model for Maternal Behavior. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1959, 59, 226–235. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 15. Lamborn, S.D.; Mounts, N.S.; Steinberg, L.; Dornbusch, S.M. Patterns of Competence and Adjustment among Adolescents from Authoritative, Authoritarian, Indulgent, and Neglectful Families. Child Dev. 1991, 62, 1049–1065. [CrossRef] 16. Garcia, F.; Gracia, E. The indulgent parenting style and developmental outcomes in South European and Latin American countries. In Parenting across Cultures; Selin, H., Ed.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2014; pp. 419–433. 17. Symonds, P.M. The Psychology of Parent-Child Relationships; Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, NY, USA, 1939. 18. Baldwin, A.L. Behaviour and Development in Childhood; Dryden Press: New York, NY, USA, 1955. 19. Freud, S. New Introductory Lectures on Psycho-Analysis; Hogarth Press: London, UK, 1933. 20. Rogers, C.R. A Therapist’s View of Personal Goals. (Pendle Hill Pamphlet 108); Pendle Hill: Oxford, UK, 1960. 21. Becker, W.C.; Krug, R.S. A Circumplex Model for Social-Behavior in Children. Child Dev. 1964, 35, 371–396. [CrossRef] 22. Sears, R.R.; Maccoby, E.E.; Levin, H. Patterns of Child Rearing; Row, Peterson: Evanston, IL, USA, 1957; p. 549. 23. Watson, J.B. Psychological Care of Infant and Child; G. Allen & Unwin: London, UK, 1928. 24. Steinberg, L. Psychological control: Style or substance? In New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development: Changes in Parental Authority during Adolescence; Smetana, J.G., Ed.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2005; pp. 71–78. 25. Garcia, O.F. Parental Socialization in Spain Across the Life Span. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain, 2019. Available online: https://hdl.handle.net/10550/72456 (accessed on 1 June 2020). 26. Axpe, I.; Rodri guez-Fernandez, A.; Goni, E.; Antonio-Agirre, I. Parental Socialization Styles: The Contribution of Paternal and Maternal a ect/communication and Strictness to Family Socialization Style. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2204. [CrossRef] 27. Alonso-Stuyck, P. Which Parenting Style Encourages Healthy Lifestyles in Teenage Children? Proposal for a Model of Integrative Parenting Styles. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2057. [CrossRef] 28. Gómez-Ortiz, O.; Romera, E.M.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Del Rey, R. Parenting Practices as Risk Or Preventive Factors for Adolescent Involvement in Cyberbullying: Contribution of Children and Parent Gender. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 2664. [CrossRef] 29. Barber, B.K.; Olsen, J.E.; Shagle, S.C. Associations between Parental Psychological and Behavioral-Control and Youth Internalized and Externalized Behaviors. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 1120–1136. [CrossRef] 30. Silk, J.S.; Morris, A.S.; Kanaya, T.; Steinberg, L. Psychological Control and Autonomy Granting: Opposite Ends of a Continuum or Distinct Constructs? J. Res. Adolesc. 2003, 13, 113–128. [CrossRef] 31. Grusec, J.E.; Lytton, H. Social Development: History, Theory, and Research; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1988. 32. Garcia, F.; Fernández-Doménech, L.; Veiga, F.H.; Bono, R.; Serra, E.; Musitu, G. Parenting styles and parenting practices: Analyzing current relationships in the Spanish context. In Parenting: Cultural Influences and Impact Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 14 of 17 on Childhood Health and Well-Being; Garcia, F., Ed.; Nova Science Publishers, Inc.: Hauppauge, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 17–31. 33. Tur-Porcar, A.; Jimenez-Martinez, J.; Mestre-Escriva, V. Substance use in Early and Middle Adolescence. The Role of Academic Ecacy and Parenting. Psychosoc. Interv. 2019, 28, 139–145. [CrossRef] 34. Lewis, C.C. The E ects of Parental Firm Control: A Reinterpretation of Findings. Psychol. Bull. 1981, 90, 547–563. [CrossRef] 35. Baumrind, D. Rejoinder to Lewis Reinterpretation of Parental Firm Control E ects: Are Authoritative Families really Harmonious? Psychol. Bull. 1983, 94, 132–142. [CrossRef] 36. Kerr, M.; Stattin, H. What Parents Know, how they Know it, and several Forms of Adolescent Adjustment: Further Support for a Reinterpretation of Monitoring. Dev. Psychol. 2000, 36, 366–380. [CrossRef] 37. Stattin, H.; Kerr, M. Parental Monitoring: A Reinterpretation. Child Dev. 2000, 71, 1072–1085. [CrossRef] 38. Steinberg, L.; Lamborn, S.D.; Dornbusch, S.M.; Darling, N. Impact of Parenting Practices on Adolescent Achievement: Authoritative Parenting, School Involvement, and Encouragement to Succeed. Child Dev. 1992, 63, 1266–1281. [CrossRef] 39. Barber, B.K. Parental Psychological Control: Revisiting a Neglected Construct. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 3296–3319. [CrossRef] 40. Arrindell, W.A.; Akkerman, A.; Bagés, N.; Feldman, L.; Caballo, V.E.; Oei, T.P.S.; Torres, B.; Canalda, G.; Castro, J.; Montgomery, I.; et al. The Short-EMBU in Australia, Spain, and Venezuela. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2005, 21, 56–66. [CrossRef] 41. Perris, C.; Jacobsson, L.; Lindstrom, H.; Knorring, L.V.; Perris, H. Development of a New Inventory for Assessing Memories of Parental Rearing Behavior. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1980, 61, 265–274. [CrossRef] 42. Arrindell, W.A.; Sanavio, E.; Aguilar, G.; Sica, C.; Hatzichristou, C.; Eisemann, M.; Recinos, L.A.; Gaszner, P.; Peter, M.; Batagliese, G.; et al. The Development of a Short Form of the EMBU: Its Appraisal with Students in Greece, Guatemala, Hungary and Italy. Pers. Individ. Di er. 1999, 27, 613–628. [CrossRef] 43. Tur-Porcar, A.; Mestre, V.; Llorca, A. Parenting: Psychometric Analysis of Two Studies in Spanish Population. Anu. Psicol. 2015, 45, 347–359. 44. Schaefer, E.S. Childrens Reports of Parental Behavior: An Inventory. Child Dev. 1965, 36, 413–424. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 45. Chao, R.K. Beyond Parental Control and Authoritarian Parenting Style: Understanding Chinese Parenting through the Cultural Notion of Training. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 1111–1119. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 46. Chao, R.K. Extending Research on the Consequences of Parenting Style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 1832–1843. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 47. Zayas, L.H.; Solari, F. Early-Childhood Socialization in Hispanic Families: Context, Culture, and Practice Implications. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 1994, 25, 200–206. [CrossRef] 48. Torres-Villa, M.S. Parenting Styles, Language and Parents’ Education as Predictors of School Achievement for Hispanic Students. Ph.D. Thesis, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA, 1995. 49. Baumrind, D. An Exploratory Study of Socialization E ects on Black Children: Some Black-White Comparisons. Child Dev. 1972, 43, 261–267. [CrossRef] 50. Deater-Deckard, K.; Dodge, K.A.; Bates, J.E.; Pettit, G.S. Physical Discipline among African American and European American Mothers: Links to Children’s Externalizing Behaviors. Dev. Psychol. 1996, 32, 1065–1072. [CrossRef] 51. Dwairy, M.; Achoui, M. Introduction to Three Cross-Regional Research Studies on Parenting Styles, Individuation, and Mental Health in Arab Societies. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2006, 37, 221–229. [CrossRef] 52. Dwairy, M.; Achoui, M.; Abouserfe, R.; Farah, A. Parenting Styles, Individuation, and Mental Health of Arab Adolescents: A Third Cross-Regional Research Study. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2006, 37, 262–272. [CrossRef] 53. Garcia, F.; Serra, E.; Garcia, O.F.; Martinez, I.; Cruise, E. A Third Emerging Stage for the Current Digital Society? Optimal Parenting Styles in Spain, the United States, Germany, and Brazil. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2333. [CrossRef] 54. Calafat, A.; Garcia, F.; Juan, M.; Becoña, E.; Fernández-Hermida, J.R. Which Parenting Style is More Protective against Adolescent Substance use? Evidence within the European Context. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014, 138, 185–192. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 55. Martínez, I.; Garcia, J.F.; Yubero, S. Parenting Styles and Adolescents’ Self-Esteem in Brazil. Psychol. Rep. 2007, 100, 731–745. [CrossRef] [PubMed] Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 15 of 17 56. Martínez, I.; Garcia, J.F. Internalization of Values and Self-Esteem among Brazilian Teenagers from Authoritative, Indulgent, Authoritarian, and Neglectful Homes. Adolescence 2008, 43, 13–29. [PubMed] 57. Villarejo, S.; Martinez-Escudero, J.A.; Garcia, O.F. Parenting Styles and their Contribution to Children Personal and Social Adjustment. Ansiedad Estrés 2020, 26, 1–8. [CrossRef] 58. Garcia, O.F.; Lopez-Fernandez, O.; Serra, E. Raising Spanish Children with an Antisocial Tendency: Do we Know what the Optimal Parenting Style is? J. Interpers. Violence 2018. [CrossRef] 59. Martínez, I.; Murgui, S.; Garcia, O.F.; Garcia, F. Parenting in the Digital Era: Protective and Risk Parenting Styles for Traditional Bullying and Cyberbullying Victimization. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 90, 84–92. [CrossRef] 60. Riquelme, M.; Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E. Psychosocial Maladjustment in Adolescence: Parental Socialization, Self-Esteem, and Substance use. An. Psicol. 2018, 34, 536–544. [CrossRef] 61. Alonso-Geta, P.M.P. Parenting Style in Spanish Parents with Children Aged 6 to 14. Psicothema 2012, 24, 371–376. 62. Fuentes, M.C.; Alarcón, A.; Gracia, E.; Garcia, F. School Adjustment among Spanish Adolescents: Influence of Parental Socialization. Cult. Educ. 2015, 27, 1–32. [CrossRef] 63. Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E.; Zacares, J.J.; Garcia, F. Parenting Styles and Short- and Long-Term Socialization Outcomes: A Study among Spanish Adolescents and Older Adults. Psychosoc. Interv. 2018, 27, 153–161. [CrossRef] 64. Musitu-Ferrer, D.; León-Moreno, C.; Callejas-Jerónimo, E.J.; Esteban-Ibáñez, M.; Musitu-Ochoa, G. Relationships between Parental Socialization Styles, Empathy and Connectedness with Nature: Their Implications in Environmentalism. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2461. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 65. Martínez, I.; Fuentes, M.; Garcia, F.; Madrid, I. The Parenting Style as Protective or Risk Factor for Substance use and Other Behavior Problems among Spanish Adolescents. Adicciones 2013, 25, 235–242. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 66. Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E.; Zacares, J.J.; Calafat, A.; Garcia, F. Alcohol use and Abuse and Motivations for Drinking and Non-Drinking among Spanish Adolescents: Do we Know enough when we Know Parenting Style? Psychol. Health 2020, 35, 645–664. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 67. Perez-Gramaje, A.F.; Garcia, O.F.; Reyes, M.; Serra, E.; Garcia, F. Parenting Styles and Aggressive Adolescents: Relationships with Self-Esteem and Personal Maladjustment. Eur. J. Psychol. Appl. Leg. Context 2020, 12, 1–10. [CrossRef] 68. Garaigordobil, M.; Aliri, J. Parental Socialization Styles, Parents’ Educational Level, and Sexist Attitudes in Adolescence. Span. J. Psychol. 2012, 15, 592–603. [CrossRef] 69. Moreno-Ruiz, D.; Martinez-Ferrer, B.; Garcia-Bacete, F. Parenting Styles, Cyberaggression, and Cybervictimization among Adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 93, 252–259. [CrossRef] 70. Chumakov, M.; Chumakova, D. Parents’ Personality, Marriage Satisfaction, Stress, and Punishment of Children in the Family. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 153. [CrossRef] 71. Garcia, F.; Gracia, E. Is always Authoritative the Optimum Parenting Style? Evidence from Spanish Families. Adolescence 2009, 44, 101–131. 72. Pinquart, M.; Gerke, D.C. Associations of Parenting Styles with Self-Esteem in Children and Adolescents: A Meta-Analysis. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2019, 28, 2017–2035. [CrossRef] 73. Pinquart, M.; Kauser, R. Do the Associations of Parenting Styles with Behavior Problems and Academic Achievement Vary by Culture? Results from a Meta-Analysis. Cult. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 2018, 24, 75–100. [CrossRef] 74. Perez-Aranda, G.I.; Peralta-Lopez, V.; Estrada-Carmona, S.; Garcia Reyes, L.; Angel Tuz-Sierra, M. Attachment and Adulthood in a Sample of Southeastern Mexico. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 134. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 75. Flotskaya, N.; Bulanova, S.; Ponomareva, M.; Flotskiy, N.; Konopleva, T. Self-Identity Development among Indigenous Adolescents from the Far North of Russia. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 106. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 76. Sta ord, M.; Kuh, D.L.; Gale, C.R.; Mishra, G.; Richards, M. Parent-Child Relationships and O spring’s Positive Mental Wellbeing from Adolescence to Early Older Age. J. Posit. Psychol. 2016, 11, 326–337. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 77. Barthomew, K.; Horowitz, L. Attachment Styles among Young-Adults—A Test of a 4-Category Model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1991, 61, 226–244. [CrossRef] 78. Baltes, P. Theoretical Propositions of Life-Span Developmental-Psychology—On the Dynamics between Growth and Decline. Dev. Psychol. 1987, 23, 611–626. [CrossRef] Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 16 of 17 79. Reigal, R.E.; Moral-Campillo, L.; Morillo-Baro, J.P.; Juarez-Ruiz de Mier, R.; Hernandez-Mendo, A.; Morales-Sanchez, V. Physical Exercise, Fitness, Cognitive Functioning, and Psychosocial Variables in an Adolescent Sample. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1100. [CrossRef] 80. Guarnido, A.J.S.; Cabrera, F.J.H.; Osuna, M.J.P. Eating Disorder Detection through Personality Traits and Self-Concept. Eat. Weight Disord. Stud. Anorex. Bulim. Obes. 2012, 17, e309–e313. 81. Maiz, E.; Balluerka, N. Trait Anxiety and Self-Concept among Children and Adolescents with Food Neophobia. Food Res. Int. 2018, 105, 1054–1059. [CrossRef] 82. Martín-Albo, J.; Núñez, J.L.; Navarro, J.G.; Grijalvo, F. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Translation and Validation in University Students. Span. J. Psychol. 2007, 10, 458–467. [CrossRef] 83. Cornella-Font, M.; Vinas-Poch, F.; Juarez-Lopez, J.R.; Malo-Cerrato, S. Risk of Addiction: Its Prevalence in Adolescence and its Relationship with Security of Attachment and Self-Concept. Clin. Health 2020, 31, 21–25. [CrossRef] 84. Holubcikova, J.; Kolarcik, P.; Geckova, A.M.; Reijneveld, S.A.; van Dijk, J.P. The Mediating E ect of Daily Nervousness and Irritability on the Relationship between Soft Drink Consumption and Aggressive Behaviour among Adolescents. Int. J. Public Health 2015, 60, 699–706. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 85. Scholte, R.; van Lieshout, C.; van Aken, M. Perceived Relational Support in Adolescence: Dimensions, Configurations, and Adolescent Adjustment. J. Res. Adolesc. 2001, 11, 71–94. [CrossRef] 86. Eisenberg, N.; Strayer, J. Empathy and its Development; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1987; p. 406. 87. Eisenberg, N.; Eggum, N.D.; Di Giunta, L. Empathy-Related Responding: Associations with Prosocial Behavior, Aggression, and Intergroup Relations. Soc. Issue Policy Rev. 2010, 4, 143–180. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 88. Paciello, M.; Fida, R.; Tramontano, C.; Cole, E.; Cerniglia, L. Moral Dilemma in Adolescence: The Role of Values, Prosocial Moral Reasoning and Moral Disengagement in Helping Decision Making. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 10, 190–205. [CrossRef] 89. Sortheix, F.M.; Schwartz, S.H. Values that Underlie and Undermine Well-being: Variability across Countries. Eur. J. Pers. 2017, 31, 187–201. [CrossRef] 90. Balliet, D.; Joireman, J.; Daniels, D.; George-Falvy, J. Empathy and the Schwartz Value System: A Test of an Integrated Hypothesis. Individ. Di er. Res. 2008, 6, 269–279. 91. Pérez, J.F.G.; Navarro, D.F.; Llobell, J.P. Statistical Power of Solomon Design. Psicothema 1999, 11, 431–436. 92. Gracia, E.; Garcia, F.; Musitu, G. Macrosocial Determinants of Social Integration: Social-Class and Area E ect. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 5, 105–119. [CrossRef] 93. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.G. Statistical Power Analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for Correlation and Regression Analysess. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [CrossRef] 94. Rohner, R.P.; Saavedra, J.; Granum, E.O. Development and Validation of the Parental Acceptance Rejection Questionnaire: Test Manual. Cat. Sel. Doc. Psychol. 1978, 8, 17–48. 95. Ali, S.; Khaleque, A.; Rohner, R.P. Pancultural Gender Di erences in the Relation between Perceived Parental Acceptance and Psychological Adjustment of Children and Adult O spring: A Meta-Analytic Review of Worldwide Research. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2015, 46, 1059–1080. [CrossRef] 96. Rohner, R.P.; Khaleque, A. Reliability and Validity of the Parental Control Scale: A Meta-Analysis of Cross-Cultural and Intracultural Studies. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2003, 34, 643–649. [CrossRef] 97. Garcia, F.; Musitu, G. AF5: Self-Concept Form 5; TEA editions: Madrid, Spain, 1999; p. 39. 98. Fuentes, M.C.; Garcia, F.; Gracia, E.; Lila, M. Self-Concept and Drug use in Adolescence. Adicciones 2011, 23, 237–248. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 99. Murgui, S.; García, C.; García, A.; Garcia, F. Self-Concept in Young Dancers and Non-Practitioners: Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the AF5 Scale. Rev. Psicol. Deporte 2012, 21, 263–269. 100. Garcia, J.F.; Musitu, G.; Veiga, F.H. Self-Concept in Adults from Spain and Portugal. Psicothema 2006, 18, 551–556. 101. Garcia, F.; Martínez, I.; Balluerka, N.; Cruise, E.; Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E. Validation of the Five-Factor Self-Concept Questionnaire AF5 in Brazil: Testing Factor Structure and Measurement Invariance Across Language (Brazilian and Spanish), Gender, and Age. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2250. [CrossRef] 102. Garcia, J.F.; Musitu, G.; Riquelme, E.; Riquelme, P. A Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the “Autoconcepto Forma 5” Questionnaire in Young Adults from Spain and Chile. Span. J. Psychol. 2011, 14, 648–658. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 17 of 17 103. Chen, F.; Garcia, O.F.; Fuentes, M.C.; Garcia-Ros, R.; Garcia, F. Self-Concept in China: Validation of the Chinese Version of the Five-Factor Self-Concept (AF5) Questionnaire. Symmetry 2020, 12, 798. [CrossRef] 104. Garcia, F.; Gracia, E.; Zeleznova, A. Validation of the English Version of the Five-Factor Self-Concept Questionnaire. Psicothema 2013, 25, 549–555. 105. Greenberger, E.; Josselson, R.; Knerr, C.; Knerr, B. The Measurement and Structure of Psychosocial Maturity. J. Youth Adolesc. 1975, 4, 127–143. [CrossRef] 106. Schwartz, S.H. Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 25, 1–65. 107. Musitu-Ferrer, D.; Esteban Ibáñez, M.; León, C.; Garcia, O.F. Is School Adjustment Related to Environmental Empathy and Connectedness to Nature? Psychosoc. Interv. 2019, 28, 101–110. [CrossRef] 108. Veiga, F.H.; Garcia, F.; Reeve, J.; Wentzel, K.; Garcia, O.F. When Adolescents with High Self-Concept Lose their Engagement in School. Rev. Psicodidact. 2015, 20, 305–320. [CrossRef] 109. Lila, M.; Gracia, E.; Garcia, F. Ambivalent Sexism, Empathy and Law Enforcement Attitudes towards Partner Violence against Women among Male Police Ocers. Psychol. Crime Law 2013, 19, 907–919. [CrossRef] 110. Baumrind, D. E ective parenting during the early adolescent transition. In Advances in Family Research Series. Family Transitions; Cowan, P.A., Herington, E.M., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1991; pp. 111–163. 111. Babore, A.; Trumello, C.; Candelori, C.; Paciello, M.; Cerniglia, L. Depressive Symptoms, Self-Esteem and Perceived Parent-Child Relationship in Early Adolescence. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 982. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 112. Buri, J.R. Parental Authority Questionnaire. J. Pers. Assess. 1991, 57, 110–119. [CrossRef] 113. Buri, J.R.; Louiselle, P.A.; Misukanis, T.M.; Mueller, R.A. E ects of Parental Authoritarianism and Authoritativeness on Self-Esteem. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1988, 14, 271–282. 114. Flouri, E. Psychological Outcomes in Midadulthood Associated with Mother’s Child-Rearing Attitudes in Early Childhood—Evidence from the 1970 British Birth Cohort. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2004, 13, 35–41. [CrossRef] 115. Besi, M.; Sakellariou, M. Teachers’ Views on the Participation of Parents in the Transition of their Children from Kindergarten to Primary School. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 124. [CrossRef] 116. Barry, C.T.; Frick, P.J.; Grafeman, S.J. Child Versus Parent Reports of Parenting Practices: Implications for the Conceptualization of Child Behavioral and Emotional Problems. Assessment 2008, 15, 294–303. [CrossRef] 117. Alvarez-Garcia, D.; Carlos Nunez, J.; Garcia, T.; Barreiro-Collazo, A. Individual, Family, and Community Predictors of Cyber-Aggression among Adolescents. Eur. J. Psychol. Appl. Leg. Context 2018, 10, 79–88. [CrossRef] 118. Assari, S.; Gibbons, F.X.; Simons, R.L. Perceived Discrimination among Black Youth: An 18-Year Longitudinal Study. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 44. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 119. Assari, S. Race, Intergenerational Social Mobility and Stressful Life Events. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 86. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 120. Queiroz, P.; Garcia, O.F.; Garcia, F.; Zacares, J.J.; Camino, C. Self and Nature: Parental Socialization, Self-Esteem, and Environmental Values in Spanish Adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3732. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 121. Zhang, X.; Gatzke-Kopp, L.; Fosco, G.M.; Bierman, K.L. Parental Support of Self-Regulation among Children at Risk for Externalizing Symptoms: Developmental Trajectories of Physiological Regulation and Behavioral Adjustment. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 528–540. [CrossRef] 122. Maleki, M.; Mardani, A.; Chehrzad, M.M.; Dianatinasab, M.; Vaismoradi, M. Social Skills in Children at Home and in Preschool. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 74. [CrossRef] 123. Aslamazova, L.A.; Muhamedrahimov, R.J.; Vershinina, E.A. Family and Child Characteristics Associated with Foster Care Breakdown. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 160. [CrossRef] © 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png Behavioral Sciences Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute

Parental Socialization and Its Impact across the Lifespan

Loading next page...
 
/lp/multidisciplinary-digital-publishing-institute/parental-socialization-and-its-impact-across-the-lifespan-A6QBXUAOCh

References

References for this paper are not available at this time. We will be adding them shortly, thank you for your patience.

Publisher
Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute
Copyright
© 1996-2020 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated Disclaimer The statements, opinions and data contained in the journals are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publisher and the editor(s). Terms and Conditions Privacy Policy
ISSN
2076-328X
DOI
10.3390/bs10060101
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

behavioral sciences Article Parental Socialization and Its Impact across the Lifespan 1 1 2 , Jose Antonio Martinez-Escudero , Sonia Villarejo , Oscar F. Garcia * and Fernando Garcia Department of Methodology of Behavioral Sciences, Faculty of Psychology, University of Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain; jomares2@alumni.uv.es (J.A.M.-E.); sovite@alumni.uv.es (S.V.); fernando.garcia@uv.es (F.G.) Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain * Correspondence: oscar.f.garcia@uv.es Received: 16 April 2020; Accepted: 12 June 2020; Published: 16 June 2020 Abstract: Classical studies have found that parental warmth combined with parental strictness is the best parental strategy to promote children’s psychosocial development. Nevertheless, a growing set of emergent studies has questioned the benefits of parental strictness. The present study examined parental socialization and its short- and long-term impact on the psychosocial development of adolescents and adult children. The sample consisted of 2150 Spanish participants, 623 adolescents (12–18 years), 619 young adults (19–35 years), 502 middle-aged adults (35–59 years), and 406 older adults (60 years or older). Families were classified into one of four typologies (indulgent, authoritative, authoritarian, and neglectful). Psychosocial development was examined with five indicators (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). The results show a common short- and long-term pattern between parenting styles and psychosocial development: the indulgent style equaled or even surpassed the authoritative style, whereas the neglectful and authoritarian styles were associated with low scores. The present findings were discussed by considering the importance of the cultural context in family socialization. Additionally, the long-term impact of parental socialization seems to be crucial, even in adulthood. Keywords: parental socialization; adolescence; adult development 1. Introduction Parental socialization consists of parents’ influence on their children, in order to, among other objectives, encourage them to learn to inhibit actions that may be annoying or harmful to others and, at the same time, acquire behaviors that society demands, including consideration for others, self-reliance, accepting responsibility, and skills that will help them to become competent adults [1]. As socialization theorists explain, modern societies cannot rely on the ubiquitous, permanent, and indefinite presence of police or supervisors (for example, parents or caregivers) to ensure that individuals conform to social norms [2,3]. For this reason, the parents’ own socializing actions must lead the child to a certain degree of self-regulation (i.e., self-control) with regard to social norms. There comes a time when parental socialization ends: the teenager is now an adult. Unfortunately, not all children reach the socialization goals and become responsible adult members of their society [4,5]. In the study of parental socialization, Diana Baumrind defined a tripartite model based on the interaction between a ection, communication, and control, yielding three parenting styles: authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive [6]; these three parenting patterns corresponded to three modes of parental control, the authoritative control, the authoritarian control, and the lack of control (i.e., Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101; doi:10.3390/bs10060101 www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 2 of 17 Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 2 of 18 modes of parental control, the authoritative control, the authoritarian control, and the lack of control permissive control) [7,8] (see Figure 1). However, the theoretical framework proposed by Maccoby (i.e., permissive control) [7,8] (see Figure 1). However, the theoretical framework proposed by and Martin (1983) [1] became the referential model for the study of parental socialization using Maccoby and Martin (1983) [1] became the referential model for the study of parental socialization two theoretically orthogonal axes, warmth (also called responsiveness, acceptance, or a ection) and using two theoretically orthogonal axes, warmth (also called responsiveness, acceptance, or affection) strictness (also called demandingness or imposition), and four styles [9–13]. and strictness (also called demandingness or imposition), and four styles [9–13]. 1.0 Strictness 1.0 Strictness 0.8 0.8 Psychological control Behavioral control 0.6 0.6 Authoritarian Authoritative Authoritarian Authoritative 0.4 0.4 0.2 0.2 Warmth Warmth 0.0 0.0 -0.2 -0.2 -0.4 -0.4 -0.6 Permissive -0.6 Neglectful Indulgent -0.8 -0.8 -1.0 -1.0 -1.0 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 -1.0 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 (a) (b) Figure 1. (a) Theoretical tripartite model ‘Y’; Representation of the tripartite model ‘Y’ proposed Figure 1. (a) Theoretical tripartite model ‘Y’; Representation of the tripartite model ‘Y’ proposed by Diana Baumrind [7]. (b) Theoretical fourfold model based on the two orthogonal dimensions; by Diana Baumrind [7]. (b) Theoretical fourfold model based on the two orthogonal Representation of the fourfold model in which parenting practices and styles are examined within the dimensions; Representation of the fourfold model in which parenting practices and styles are two orthogonal dimensions [1,11,14–16]. examined within the two orthogonal dimensions [1,11,14–16]. It is important to note that scholars for these two orthogonal parental dimensions used di erent It is important to note that scholars for these two orthogonal parental dimensions used different labels, labe although ls, althoutheir gh their operationalization operationalizationwas was ver very y s similar imilar. Hi . Historically storically, th , e w theawarmth rmth dimens dimension ion has has been labeled by early scholars as acceptance [17], assurance [18], nurturance [19,20], warmth [21,22], been labeled by early scholars as acceptance [17], assurance [18], nurturance [19,20], warmth [21,22], or love [14]; all of these labels have a similar meanings to acceptance/involvement [15]. The strictness or love [14]; all of these labels have a similar meanings to acceptance/involvement [15]. The strictness dimensions has also been labeled as domination [17], hostility [18], inflexibility [22], control [14,23], dimensions has also been labeled as domination [17], hostility [18], inflexibility [22], control [14,23], firm firm control [24] or restriction [21]; all of these labels have a similar meanings to strictness/supervision control [24] or restriction [21]; all of these labels have a similar meanings to strictness/supervision [15]. [15]. In this sense, although schoolars have worked from different theoretical perspectives (e.g., In this sense, although schoolars have worked from di erent theoretical perspectives (e.g., behaviorist behaviorist and Freudian theory), they described a simmilar parenting patterns [3,11,25]. As and Freudian theory), they described a simmilar parenting patterns [3,11,25]. As Steinberg noted Steinberg noted (2005, p. 71), “responsiveness was often operationalized using measures of parental (2005, p. 71), “responsiveness was often operationalized using measures of parental warmth and warmth and acceptance, while demandingness came to be defined with respect to parental firmness" acceptance, while demandingness came to be defined with respect to parental firmness” [24]. The four [24]. The four parenting styles are defined by combining the two orthogonal dimensions: parenting authorit styles ative parents are defined (high warmth and high strictne by combining the two orthogonal ss), authorit dimensions: arian parentauthoritative s (low warmth an par d hi ents gh(high strictness), indulgent parents (high warmth and low strictness), and neglectful parents (low warmth warmth and high strictness), authoritarian parents (low warmth and high strictness), indulgent parents and low strictness) [26–28]. The empirical validation of the Maccoby and Martin model (1983) [1] with (high warmth and low strictness), and neglectful parents (low warmth and low strictness) [26–28]. a sample of more than 10,000 adolescents and their families [15] showed the need to distinguish The empirical validation of the Maccoby and Martin model (1983) [1] with a sample of more than between two types of "permissive" parents: indulgent (with warmth) and neglectful (without 10,000 adolescents and their families [15] showed the need to distinguish between two types of warmth). “permissive” parents: indulgent (with warmth) and neglectful (without warmth). The fourfold theoretical parenting model (i.e., authoritative, indulgent, authoritarian and The fourfold theoretical parenting model (i.e., authoritative, indulgent, authoritarian and neglectful) emphasizes the need to examine the combined effects of the two parental dimensions (i.e., neglectful) emphasizes the need to examine the combined e ects of the two parental dimensions (i.e., warmth and strictness) in order to accurately analyze the relationship between parental socialization warmth and strictness) in order to accurately analyze the relationship between parental socialization and child adjustment [15]. The two parental dimensions would express two types of persistent patterns in parental strategies, theoretically defined as independent or orthogonal constructs (they and child adjustment [15]. The two parental dimensions would express two types of persistent patterns are not related, and parenting in one of them does not allow us to know what parenting will be like in parental strategies, theoretically defined as independent or orthogonal constructs (they are not in the other). Furthermore, the fourfold theoretical parenting model also helps researchers to organize related, and parenting in one of them does not allow us to know what parenting will be like in the other). Furthermore, the fourfold theoretical parenting model also helps researchers to organize di erent parenting practices according to these parental dimensions [1,11] (see Figure 1). For example, parenting practices based on psychological control are characterized by high strictness and low warmth, which are Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 3 of 17 Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 3 of 18 related diff toerent pa authoritarian renting pra parents ctices a [29]; ccordi parenting ng to these practices parental dim of behavioral ensions contr [1, ol 11] ( arescharacterized ee Figure 1). F by or high example, parenting practices based on psychological control are characterized by high strictness and strictness and high warmth, which are related to authoritative parents [30]. Parenting practices such as low warmth, which are related to authoritarian parents [29]; parenting practices of behavioral control reasoning and dialogue to limit the child’s incorrect behaviors are positively related to the parental are characterized by high strictness and high warmth, which are related to authoritative parents [30]. warmth dimension, which is related to authoritative and indulgent parents [31]. Di erent empirical Parenting practices such as reasoning and dialogue to limit the child’s incorrect behaviors are studies have tested the organization of di erent parental practices on the two main parental dimensions positively related to the parental warmth dimension, which is related to authoritative and indulgent across di erent cultural contexts as United States [30] or Europe [32,33] (see Figure 2). In general, parents [31]. Different empirical studies have tested the organization of different parental practices the main findings from these studies agreed on the organization of di erent parental practices on on the two main parental dimensions across different cultural contexts as United States [30] or Europe the two main parental dimensions. Furthermore, the analysis of the impact of parenting practices [32,33] (see Figure 2). In general, the main findings from these studies agreed on the organization of on child different p adjustment arental is pra qui cti te ces on the two accurate in examining main parent them al dim within ensions the . Furthermor general context e, the anof alysi the s of par the enting impact of parenting practices on child adjustment is quite accurate in examining them within the style (see Darling and Steinberg, 1993) [11]. For example, monitoring was originally identified as general context of the parenting style (see Darling and Steinberg, 1993) [11]. For example, monitoring a parenting practice related to parental e orts to watch over their children as a form of strict or was originally identified as a parenting practice related to parental efforts to watch over their children firm control [34,35]. Overall, parental monitoring predicted a wide range of adjustment outcomes. as a form of strict or firm control [34,35]. Overall, parental monitoring predicted a wide range of Nevertheless, most of the relationships between monitoring and adjustment outcomes can be explained adjustment outcomes. Nevertheless, most of the relationships between monitoring and adjustment by children’s spontaneous disclosure of information to their parents (authoritative parenting), but not outcomes can be explained by children's spontaneous disclosure of information to their parents by parents’ tracking and surveillance e orts (authoritarian parenting) (see Stattin and Kerr, 2000; Kerr (authoritative parenting), but not by parents' tracking and surveillance efforts (authoritarian and Stattin, 2000) [36,37]. parenting) (see Stattin and Kerr, 2000; Kerr and Stattin, 2000) [36,37]. 1.0 1.0 Protection [s- EMBU] Strictness 0.8 Strictness 0.8 Psychological control Behavioral Psychological control Control APM 0.6 0.6 Authoritarian Authoritative Authoritarian Authoritative Rejection [s-EMBU] 0.4 0.4 0.2 0.2 Emotional Warmth [s- EMBU] Rejection 0.0 0.0 Warmth Support and Warmth communication Acceptance/ -0.2 Involvement [APM] -0.2 -0.4 -0.4 -0.6 Neglectful Indulgent -0.6 Neglectful Indulgent -0.8 -0.8 Neglect Psychological Autonomy Granting [APM] -1.0 -1.0 -1.0 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 -1.0 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 (a) (b) Figure 2. Representation of the fourfold model in which parenting practices and styles are examined Figure 2. Representation of the fourfold model in which parenting practices and styles are examined within the two orthogonal dimensions in the Spanish cultural context. (a) Garcia et al., 2015; Empirical within the two orthogonal dimensions in the Spanish cultural context. (a) Garcia et al., 2015; Empirical study of Garcia et al., 2015 [32]. (b) Tur-Porcar et al., 2019; Empirical study of Tur-Porcar et al., study of Garcia et al., 2015 [32]. (b) Tur-Porcar et al., 2019; Empirical study of Tur-Porcar et al., 2019 [APM] [BAR] [APM] [BAR] 2019 [33]. Note. The Authoritative Parenting Measure, APM [15,38]. The Psychological [33]. Note. The Authoritative Parenting Measure, APM [15,38]. The Psychological Control [s-EMBU [s-EMBU] ] ContrScale – Y ol Scale–Y outh Self-Report, outh Self-Report, PCS-YSR [39]. PCS-YSR [39]. The S(ho The rt)-EMBU [40– S(hort)-EMBU 42]. The measures of Tur- [40–42]. The measures of Porcar et al. 2019 are from Child Reports of Parental Behavior Inventory, CRPBI [43,44]. Tur-Porcar et al. 2019 are from Child Reports of Parental Behavior Inventory, CRPBI [43,44]. Empirical research on parental socialization, mainly in Anglo-Saxon cultural contexts with Empirical research on parental socialization, mainly in Anglo-Saxon cultural contexts with European-American middle-class families, indicates that authoritative parents produce children with European-American middle-class families, indicates that authoritative parents produce children better psychosocial development, compared to children from the other three types of families with better psychosocial development, compared to children from the other three types of [3,6,11,15]. However, results of studies with ethnic minorities from the United States, for example, families [3,6,11,15]. However, results of studies with ethnic minorities from the United States, for Chinese-Americans [45,46], Hispanic-Americans [47,48], or African-Americans [49,50], as well as example, Chinese-Americans [45,46], Hispanic-Americans [47,48], or African-Americans [49,50], as studies in Arab societies [51,52], question the idea that authoritative parenting (parental strictness well as studies in Arab societies [51,52], question the idea that authoritative parenting (parental with warmth) is always the most appropriate parental socialization style. These studies show that the strictness with warmth) is always the most appropriate parental socialization style. These studies show that the authoritarian style (parental strictness without warmth) is positively related to some indicators of psychosocial development in children and adolescents. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 4 of 17 Likewise, a growing set of studies, carried out mainly with families from European [53,54] and Latin-American countries [55,56], suggests that the indulgent style (parental warmth without strictness) is associated with equal or even greater psychosocial development than the authoritative style [57–60]. Children with indulgent parents, compared to their peers in authoritative families, show equal or even better adjustment and competence indices on indicators of optimal psychosocial development, such as self-concept [53,54], emotional regulation [61], school competence [62], internalization of values [63], including self-transcendence (universalism and benevolence) and conservation (security, conformity, and tradition), or connectivity with the environment [64], and protection from problems such as substance use [65], alcohol consumption [66], personal maladjustment indicators [67], and emotional maladjustment, including nervousness and hostility [58], sexist attitudes [68], and cyber-aggression and cybervictimization [69]. The parental socialization literature shows discrepant results about the relationship between parenting styles and child development, depending on the ethnic, socioeconomic, or cultural context where parental socialization takes place [9,70–73]. Additionally, although the impact of parental socialization is known to be crucial for the child, once adolescence is over and it is necessary to have the skills demanded by the adult world [1,74,75], few studies have examined long-term parental socialization beyond adolescence. Most of these studies have been limited to young adults [58], or adolescents and older adults have been compared using di erent adjustment criteria [76]. Although developmental theorists have highlighted the importance of early socialization experiences on development well beyond adolescence [3,77], less is known about the links between parenting and developmental outcomes in adulthood [76]. This may be due to the many non-normative influences in adulthood or the time elapsed since the early socialization experiences [78]. Therefore, a crucial question is whether di erences in development, in both adolescent and adult children, can be consistently related to parenting styles. In the present study, the relationship between parental socialization styles (i.e., indulgent, authoritative, authoritarian, and neglectful) and short-term and long-term psychosocial development in adolescents and adults (young, middle-aged, and older) was examined through a wide range of indicators (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Importantly, some previous parenting studies have examined these indicators of psychosocial development, but in isolation rather than simultaneously. Overall, these indicators have been identified as important for psychosocial development. Physical self-concept, which includes self-perceptions about physical appearance and physical performance, is positively related to physical fitness [79] and a protective factor against eating disorders [80] or food neophobia [81]. Family self-concept, which includes self-perceptions about feeling loved and appreciated by the family, is positively related to general self-esteem [82] or secure attachment [83]. Nervousness is an indicator of emotional instability that is linked to aggressive behaviors or alcohol consumption [84,85]. Empathy, which involves sharing someone else’s perceived emotion—“feeling with” another [86], is related to more prosocial behaviors and less aggressive behaviors [87]. The values of benevolence are part of the self-transcendence values, identified in the Swartz theoretical framework as being strongly connected to prosocial behaviors and empathy [88,89]. In particular, benevolence values might be more connected to empathy than the other nine social values proposed by Swartz [90]. Based on previous studies, children from indulgent families are expected to show equal or even better psychosocial development on all the indicators than children from authoritative families, whereas children from neglectful and authoritarian families are expected to obtain the worst adjustment on all the indicators. 2. Materials and Methods 2.1. Participants and Procedure The present study involved 2150 adolescent and adult children (M = 35.63, SD = 20.67), 1262 women (57.7%) and 888 men (58.7%): Adolescents (n = 623, 361 women, 59.7%) from 12 to 18 years old (M = 16.57; SD = 1.72); young adults (n = 619, 366 women, 59.1%) from 19 to 35 years old (M = 23.68 SD = 3.87); Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 5 of 17 middle-aged adults (n = 502, 322 women, 64.1%) from 36 to 59 years old (M = 48.30; SD = 6.32); and older adults (n = 406, 213 women, 52.5%) over 60 years old (M = 68.66: SD = 7.80). Applying an a priori power analysis [91,92], the minimum sample required was 2150 observations to detect a power of 0.89 ( = 0.05; 1 = 0.95), with a medium-low e ect size, f = 0.17, estimated value with the ANOVAs on a univariate F test of the four parenting styles [15,93]. Following previous studies on socialization with adolescent children and adult children, a similar procedure was followed for data collection in this study [5,58,63]. In short, adolescents were recruited from high schools that were selected randomly from the complete list of high schools; young adults from university undergraduate courses; middle aged-adults from city council neighborhoods; and older adults from senior citizen centers that were selected randomly from the complete list of senior citizen centers. Data collection was conducted during the 2017–2018 and 2018–2019 academic years. All the participants in this study: (a) were Spanish, as were their parents and their four grandparents; (b) lived in nuclear families with two parents, a mother or female primary caregiver and a father or male primary caregiver; (c) participated voluntarily, both adolescents and adult children, with the prior consent of their parents to participate in the case of the adolescents; (d) filled out the self-report paper-and-pencil questionnaires. According to the Declaration of Helsinki, the protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Program for the Promotion of Scientific Research, Technological Development, and Innovation of the Spanish Valencian Region, the public institution that supports the present research. This study was also approved by the College Research Ethics Committee (CREC) of the Nottingham Trent University (NTU, Nottingham, UK; Project No. 2017/90). 2.2. Measures 2.2.1. Parental Socialization The parental warmth dimension was measured with the 20 items on the Warmth/A ection Scale, originally developed by Rohner (1978) [94] and widely used in parenting studies [95]. This scale o ers a reliable measure of the degree to which children experience involvement and a ection from their parents (Examples of items: “Say nice things to me when I deserve them” and “Are really interested in what I do”), and it also has an adult version (Examples of items: “Said nice things to me when I deserved it” and “Were really interested in what I did”). An alpha coecient of 0.945 was obtained. The parental strictness dimension was measured with the 13 items on the Parental Control Scale, originally developed by Rohner (1978) [94], and widely used in parenting studies [96]. This scale o ers a reliable measure of the degree to which children experience control with firmness, demand, and severity by their parents (Examples of items: “Give me certain jobs to do and will not let me do anything else until I am done” and “Want to control whatever I do”), and it also has an adult version (Examples of items: “Gave me certain jobs to do and would not let me do anything else until I was done” and “Wanted to control whatever I did”). An alpha coecient of 0.904 was obtained. Both parental measures use a Likert-type response scale that ranges from 1 “almost never true” to 4 “almost always true”. Higher scores on the two parental measures represent high parental warmth and strictness. The four parenting styles were defined following the dichotomization procedure based on the median of the scores on each of the two parental dimensions (i.e., warmth and strictness) and examining them simultaneously: Authoritative families scored above the median on both parental dimensions, neglectful families scored below the median on both parental dimensions, indulgent families scored above the median on parental warmth and below the median on parental strictness, and authoritarian families scored below the median on parental warmth and above the median on parental strictness [5,15,46]. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 6 of 17 2.2.2. Psychosocial Development Physical and family self-concept. These two dimensions of self-concept were measured with the family and physical scales of the AF5 Self-Concept Questionnaire [97]. The AF5 is a widely validated questionnaire in samples of adolescents and adults [98–100] in several countries, such as Spain [99], Portugal [100], Brazil [101], Chile [102], China [103], or the United States [104]. The AF5 theoretical factorial structure (i.e. multidimensional) is invariant across Western and non-Western societies [101,103,104]. The physical component refers to the individual’s perception of his/her condition and physical appearance (Example item: “I like the way I look). An alpha coecient of 0.782 was obtained. The family component refers to the individual’s perception of his/her integration, involvement, and participation in the family environment (Example item: “My family would help me with any type of problem”). An alpha coecient of 0.816 was obtained. Both measures of self-concept, with six items each, use a Likert-type response scale ranging from 1 “Strong disagreement” to 99 “Strong agreement”. Higher scores on both measures indicate a higher sense of family and physical self-concept. Nervousness. It was measured with the 8 items on the nervousness scale of the Psychosocial Maturity Questionnaire [5,58,105]. Nervousness refers to the lack of emotional stability and anxiety in situations in everyday life (Example item: “My mood changes easily”). The scale uses a Likert-type response format ranging from 1 “Strongly disagree” to 5 “Strongly agree”. Higher scores on this scale indicate a higher degree of nervousness. An alpha coecient of 0.778 was obtained. Empathy. It was measured with the 5 items on the empathy scale of the Psychosocial Maturity Questionnaire [5,58,105]. Empathy refers to understanding others and considering other views apart from one’s own (Example item: “I am sensitive to others’ feelings and needs”). An alpha coecient of 0.672 was obtained. The scale uses a Likert-type response format ranging from 1 “Strongly disagree” to 5 “Strongly agree”. High scores on this scale indicate a high degree of empathy. Internalization of social values. The benevolence values were measured with the 5 benevolence scale items on the Schwartz Value Inventory [89,106]. The values of benevolence refer to the care of family relationships and values such as forgiveness (Example item: “Forgiving (Willing to pardon others)”). An alpha coecient of 0.740 was obtained. The scale uses a Likert-type response format ranging from 1 “Opposed to my values” to 99 “of supreme importance”. Scores on this scale indicate that a high priority is given to benevolence values. 2.3. Plan of Analysis A MANOVA (multivariate factorial analysis of variance) (4  2  4) was performed on the five indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence), with parenting styles (authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful), sex (women vs. men), and age (adolescents, young adults, middle-aged adults, and older adults) as independent variables. Univariate F tests were applied to all the sources of variation where statistically significant di erences were found on multivariate tests. The significant univariate results were followed by post-hoc Bonferroni comparisons for all the possible pairs of averages [5,107,108]. 3. Results 3.1. Parenting Styles Participants were distributed in the four family types (see Table 1). On parental warmth, children from indulgent (M = 73.47; SD = 4.63) and authoritative (M = 72.45, SD = 4.37) families scored higher than children from authoritarian (M = 55.00; SD = 10.04) and neglectful (M = 57.04; SD = 9.52) families. On parental strictness, the children from authoritative (M = 40.05, SD = 5.00) and authoritarian (M = 41.90; SD = 5.52) families scored higher than the children from indulgent (M = 28.35, SD = 5.57) and neglectful (M = 28.25, SD = 5.79). The results from the correlation analysis confirmed the Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 7 of 17 orthogonal theoretical assumption; the correlation between the two parental dimensions (i.e., warmth and strictness) was modest, r = 0.268, R = 0.072, less than 8%, p < 0.001 (the range in the shared variance, i.e., R , is from 0 to 1). Table 1. Distribution of participants by parenting style. Total Indulgent Authoritative Authoritarian Neglectful Warmth Mean 64.48 73.47 72.45 55.00 57.04 SD 1.52 4.63 4.37 10.04 9.52 Strictness Mean 34.69 28.35 40.05 41.90 28.25 SD 8.46 5.57 5.00 5.52 5.79 N (%) 2150 (100%) 628 (29.20%) 451 (21.00) 620 (28.80%) 451 (21.00%) 3.2. MANOVAs Multivariate Analysis Statistically significant di erences were obtained for the main e ects of parenting style, L = 0.742, F(15, 5836.2) = 44.45, p < 0.001; sex, L = 0.881, F(5, 2114.0) = 57.34, p < 0.001; and age, L = 0.895, F(15, 5836.2), = 16.02, p < 0.001 (see Table 2). The interaction e ects between parenting styles and sex also reached the level of statistical significance, L = 0.985, F(15, 5836.2) = 2.17, p = 0.006; as did the e ects between parenting styles and age, L = 0.958, F(45, 10562.0) = 2.00, p < 0.001; and between sex and age, L = 0.987, F(15, 5836.2) = 1.87, p = 0.021. a b c Table 2. Multivariate factorial analysis of variance (4  2  3 ) for the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values). Source of Variation L F df df p between error (A) Parenting Style 0.742 44.45 15 5836.2 <0.001 0.881 57.34 5 2114.0 <0.001 (B) Sex (C) Age 0.895 16.02 15 5836.2 <0.001 A  B 0.985 2.17 15 5836.2 0.006 A  C 0.958 2.00 45 9459.5 <0.001 B  C 0.987 1.87 15 5836.2 0.021 A  B  C 0.974 1.23 45 9459.5 0.138 a b c a , indulgent, a , authoritative, a , authoritarian, a , neglectful; b , male, b , female; c , adolescents, c , 1 2 3 4 1 2 1 2 young adults, c , middle-aged adults, c , older adults. 3 4 3.3. Parenting Styles On the psychosocial adjustment criteria, the indulgent style was related to equal or even better scores than the authoritative style, whereas the lowest scores corresponded to the neglectful and authoritarian styles (see Table 3). (i) On physical self-concept, an interaction effect between parenting style and sex was found, F(3, 2118) = 2.82, p = 0.037 (see Figure 3). Although women always scored lower than men, a similar pattern was seen when examining parenting styles. In men, indulgent and authoritative styles were related to better physical self-concept than neglectful and authoritarian styles. In women, indulgent and authoritative styles were associated with the highest scores, whereas the authoritarian style was associated with the lowest scores. (ii) On family self-concept, an interaction effect between parenting style and age was found, F(9, 2118) = 5.49, p < 0.001 (see Figure 1). The indulgent style was related to higher scores than the authoritative style, a trend that was observed within the groups of teenagers and young adults. The authoritative style was associated with higher scores than the authoritarian and neglectful styles. Finally, the lowest scores on family self-concept were related to the authoritarian style, a trend that was observed in the groups of teenagers and young adults, but not in middle-aged adults, whereas in the group of older adults, the lowest scores corresponded to the neglectful style. (iii) In the case of nervousness, the children from indulgent and authoritative families obtained lower scores than Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 8 of 17 their peers from authoritarian and neglectful households. (iv) For empathy, an interaction effect between parenting style and age was found, F(9, 2118) = 2.80, p = 0.003 (see Figure 1). Among the adolescents, the indulgent style was related to the highest scores on empathy, and the lowest scores corresponded to the authoritarian and neglectful parenting styles, with the authoritative style in an intermediate position. Similarly, in adult children, the indulgent and authoritative styles were associated with the highest scores (in all three groups), whereas the lowest scores corresponded to the authoritarian (in young adults) and neglectful (in young, middle-aged, and older adults) styles. (v) On the internalization of social values of benevolence, children with indulgent and authoritative parents reported higher scores than children from authoritarian and neglectful families. Table 3. Means, standard deviations (in parenthesis), F values, and Bonferroni’s test across parenting styles on the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Psychosocial Indulgent Authoritative Authoritarian Neglectful F(1, 2118) p Development 1 1 2 2 Physical self-concept 5.94 6.15 5.39 5.61 18.10 <0.001 (1.74) (1.79) (1.83) (1.68) 1 2 4 3 Family self-concept 8.74 8.51 7.13 7.41 212.01 <0.001 (0.95) (1.04) (1.57) (1.50) 2 2 1 1 Nervousness 2.20 2.29 2.56 2.49 38.06 <0.001 (0.63) (0.61) (0.64) (0.63) 1 1 2 2 Empathy 55.32 <0.001 4.11 4.05 3.81 3.74 (0.50) (0.52) (0.61) (0.63) 1 1 2 2 Social values 8.26 8.20 7.80 7.67 40.33 <0.001 of benevolence (1.04) (1.03) (1.20) (1.21) Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 9 of 18 Note: Bonferroni test = 0.05; 1 > 2 > 3 > 4. 7.5 9.5 Sex Parenting styles 9.04 9.0 7.0 8.90 Indulgent Women Authoritative 6.71 8.58 8.5 8.51 8.74 6.5 8.26 Authoritarian 6.28 Men 8.43 8.24 Neglectful 8.0 6.0 5.86 5.66 5.86 7.75 7.70 5.72 7.5 5.5 7.27 7.18 5.44 7.14 7.0 7.11 7.00 5.07 5.0 6.74 6.5 4.5 Adole s ce nts Young adults M iddle -age d Olde r adults Indulge nt Authoritative Authortiarian Ne ge lctful adults Parenting styles Age (a) (b) Parenting styles 4.16 4.2 4.10 4.12 4.1 Indulgent 4.12 4.03 4.09 Authoritative 4.0 4.02 3.94 3.90 Authoritarian 3.92 3.9 Neglectful 3.87 3.8 3.77 3.77 3.7 3.70 3.70 3.6 3.5 3.50 3.4 Adole s ce nts Young adults Middle -age d Olde r adults adults Age (c) Figure 3. Interaction e ects between parenting style and sex. (a) Physical self-concept. Interaction e ects Figure 3. Interaction effects between parenting style and sex. (a) Physical self-concept. Interaction between par effects between enting style parenting styl and age. ( e and age. ( b) Family b)self-concept, Family self-conc and ept, and (c) ( Empathy c) Empath. y. 3.4. Sex-and-Age-Related Differences Statistically significant sex differences were found on the five psychosocial development criteria examined (see Table 4). On physical self-concept, men scored higher than women. On family self- concept, an interaction between sex and age was found, F(3, 2118) = 3.79, p = 0.010 (see Figure 4). Women indicated a higher family self-concept than men, a trend that was observed in adolescents and young adults and, to a lesser extent, in middle-aged adults (in older adults, men and women scored the same). On nervousness, empathy, and social values of benevolence, women scored higher than men. Table 4. Means, standard deviations (in parenthesis), F values, and Bonferroni’s test between women and men on the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Psychosocial Development Women Men F(1, 2118) p Physical self-concept 5.46 6.18 83.57 < 0.001 (1.76) (1.75) Family self-concept 8.07 7.76 21.11 < 0.001 (1.44) (1.51) Nervousness 4.04 3.78 104.42 < 0.001 (0.57) (0.58) Empathy 2.44 2.31 22.13 < 0.001 Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 9 of 17 3.4. Sex-and-Age-Related Di erences Statistically significant sex di erences were found on the five psychosocial development criteria examined (see Table 4). On physical self-concept, men scored higher than women. On family self-concept, an interaction between sex and age was found, F(3, 2118) = 3.79, p = 0.010 (see Figure 4). Women indicated a higher family self-concept than men, a trend that was observed in adolescents and young adults and, to a lesser extent, in middle-aged adults (in older adults, men and women scored the same). On nervousness, empathy, and social values of benevolence, women scored higher than men. Table 4. Means, standard deviations (in parenthesis), F values, and Bonferroni’s test between women and men on the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Psychosocial Women Men F(1, 2118) p Development Physical 5.46 6.18 83.57 <0.001 self-concept (1.76) (1.75) Family self-concept 8.07 7.76 21.11 <0.001 (1.44) (1.51) Nervousness 4.04 3.78 104.42 <0.001 Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 10 of 18 (0.57) (0.58) Empathy 2.44 2.31 22.13 <0.001 (0.66) (0.62) (0.66) (0.62) Social values 8.13 7.79 42.94 < 0.001 Social values 8.13 7.79 42.94 <0.001 of benevolence (1.12) (1.16) of benevolence (1.12) (1.16) Note: Bonferroni test α = 0.05. Note: Bonferroni test = 0.05. 8.5 Sex 8.4 8.27 8.3 Women 8.24 8.2 8.1 Men 8.0 7.93 7.92 7.9 7.8 7.74 7.73 7.7 7.67 7.6 7.62 7.5 Adole sce nts Young adults M iddle-age d Olde r adults adults Age Figure 4. Interaction between sex and age. Family self-concept. Figure 4. Interaction between sex and age. Family self-concept. Statistically significant di erences in age were found on the five psychosocial development criteria examined (see Table 5). On physical self-concept, adolescents and young adults obtained higher Statistically significant differences in age were found on the five psychosocial development scores than middle-aged adults, who, in turn, scored higher than older adults. On family self-concept, criteria examined (see Table 5). On physical self-concept, adolescents and young adults obtained examining the sex profile by age (see Figure 4), the highest scores in women corresponded to the age higher scores than middle-aged adults, who, in turn, scored higher than older adults. On family self- group of adolescents and young adults, whereas the men who scored the highest were the young adults. concept, examining the sex profile by age (see Figure 4), the highest scores in women corresponded Onnervousness, adolescents and young adults scored higher than middle-aged and older adults. to the age group of adolescents and young adults, whereas the men who scored the highest were the On empathy, young adults and middle-aged adults reported higher scores than adolescents and young adults. On nervousness, adolescents and young adults scored higher than middle-aged and older adults. On priority given to benevolence values, middle-aged adults and older adults obtained older adults. On empathy, young adults and middle-aged adults reported higher scores than the highest scores, whereas the lowest scores corresponded to adolescents (older adults exceeded adolescents and older adults. On priority given to benevolence values, middle-aged adults and older young adults). adults obtained the highest scores, whereas the lowest scores corresponded to adolescents (older adults exceeded young adults). Table 5. Means, standard deviations (in parenthesis), F values, and Bonferroni’s test across age groups on the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Psychosocial Young Middle-aged Older Adolescents F(3, 2118) p Development Adults Adults Adults 1 1 2 3 Physical self-concept 6.13 5.96 5.54 5.15 33.72 < 0.001 (1.83) (1.78) (1.71) (1.65) a 1 2 2. b Family self-concept 8.03 8.13 7.86 7.65 13.83 < 0.001 (1.49) (1.46) (1.43) (1.48) 1 1 2 2 Nervousness 2.44 2.40 2.33 2.34 2.69 < 0.001 (0.64) (0.65) (0.65) (0.64) 2.b 1 1. a 2 Empathy 3.89 4.01 3.96 3.84 8.10 < 0.001 (0.57) (0.55) (0.58) (0.64) 2 b 1 1. a Social values 7.78 8.00 8.19 8.04 12.19 < 0.001 of benevolence (1.23) (1.09) (1.05) (1.18) Note: Bonferroni test α = .05; 1 > 2 > 3; a > b. 4. Discussion This paper examined the relationship between parenting styles (indulgent, authoritative, authoritarian, and neglectful) and psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of benevolence social values) in the short term in teenagers, and in the long term in adult children (young adults, middle-aged adults, and older Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 10 of 17 Table 5. Means, standard deviations (in parenthesis), F values, and Bonferroni’s test across age groups on the indicators of psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of social values of benevolence). Psychosocial Young Middle-aged Older Adolescents F(3, 2118) p Development Adults Adults Adults Physical 1 1 2 3 6.13 5.96 5.54 5.15 33.72 <0.001 self-concept (1.83) (1.78) (1.71) (1.65) Family a 1 2 2,b 8.03 8.13 7.86 7.65 13.83 <0.001 self-concept (1.49) (1.46) (1.43) (1.48) 1 1 2 2 Nervousness 2.44 2.40 2.33 2.34 2.69 <0.001 (0.64) (0.65) (0.65) (0.64) 2,b 1 1,a 2 Empathy 3.89 4.01 3.96 3.84 8.10 <0.001 (0.57) (0.55) (0.58) (0.64) 2 b 1 1,a Social values 12.19 <0.001 7.78 8.00 8.19 8.04 of benevolence (1.23) (1.09) (1.05) (1.18) Note: Bonferroni test = 0.05; 1 > 2 > 3; a > b. 4. Discussion This paper examined the relationship between parenting styles (indulgent, authoritative, authoritarian, and neglectful) and psychosocial development (physical and family self-concept, nervousness, empathy, and internalization of benevolence social values) in the short term in teenagers, and in the long term in adult children (young adults, middle-aged adults, and older adults). The measures of the parental dimensions (i.e., warmth and strictness) were modestly correlated, confirming the orthogonality theoretical assumption. The results of this study show a common pattern in the short and long term in the relationship between parenting styles and psychosocial development. The indulgent style (parental warmth without strictness) appears to be the optimal style, associated with equal or even better results than authoritative parenting (parental warmth with strictness), whereas the styles with a lack of parental warmth (authoritarian and neglectful) are related to low scores on the psychosocial development criteria examined. In general, the results of the present study indicate that the indulgent and authoritative styles were related to better psychosocial adjustment indicators than the authoritarian and neglectful styles. In the case of physical self-concept, examining the profile of parenting styles by sex, we observed that, although men showed a higher physical self-concept than women, parenting styles characterized by warmth (indulgent and authoritative) were related to a higher physical self-concept in both men and women. With regard to family self-concept, examining the profile of parenting styles by age, we observed that the indulgent style was related to a higher family self-concept than the authoritative style (in adolescents and young adults), and the authoritative style was associated with a higher family self-concept than the authoritarian style. Regarding the parenting styles without parental warmth, children raised in authoritarian families obtained the lowest family self-concept scores (although this trend was only observed in adolescents and young adults). In the case of empathy, the profile of parenting styles by age revealed that the indulgent style was related to equal or better results (in adolescents), whereas neglectful and authoritarian parenting styles were related to less empathy (in middle-aged adult children and older adults, those who were raised by neglectful families indicated the lowest empathy). In relation to benevolence, parenting styles characterized by warmth (indulgent and authoritative) gave a higher priority to this social value than parenting styles characterized by lack of warmth (neglectful and authoritarian). An important question that has been widely examined in the parenting literature for years has to do with the best parenting strategy to promote optimal psychosocial development [5,6,11]. The influence of parents on children is usually captured through parental warmth and strictness, both defined as orthogonal (i.e., unrelated) dimension [1,11]. The results from the present study confirm other previous Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 11 of 17 research from studies mainly conducted in European [53,54,57] and Latin-American countries [9,55,56], where indulgent parenting (i.e., warmth without strictness) is related to equal or even better results than authoritative parenting (i.e., warmth with strictness), thus extending the evidence about the benefits of indulgent parenting to five important indicators of psychosocial development. Nevertheless, the present findings also contradict the commonly accepted idea about the benefits of parental strictness combined with parental warmth for child development, based mainly on results from studies conducted with middle-class European-American families [6,11,15]. In these studies, children and adolescents from authoritative families have greater psychosocial development on di erent indicators than their peers from the other households [1,4,24]. It seems that children from families characterized by warmth and strictness (i.e., authoritative parenting) do not always develop the best competence and adjustment in all cultural contexts. For example, some previous research in ethnic minorities from the United States [46,49] as well as from Asian and Arab countries [51,52], revealed some benefits related to authoritarian parenting (i.e., warmth without strictness). Therefore, a particularly pressing issue is the di erent impact of the parenting styles depending on the child’s cultural background [11,71]. Another main contributions of this study is that it provides empirical evidence about the consistent links between parenting and psychosocial development, not only in the short term in adolescent children, but also in the long term, beyond adolescence, when parental socialization has ended, using five indicators of key psychosocial development in adolescence and adult life: the individual’s perception of him/herself in the physical and family environment (physical and family self-concept), the individual’s emotional stability (captured with an indicator of mismatch, that is, nervousness), the ability to understand and understand others (empathy), and the internalization of social values (priority given to benevolence). Theorists on parental socialization point out that during the socialization process, parents exert their influence on children through practices of a ection, dialogue, reasoning (parental warmth), and firm or restrictive practices (parental strictness), in order to achieve one of the main objectives of socialization: for the child to become a responsible adult [1–3]. The findings of the present study show that, in both adolescents and adults (young, middle-aged, and older), the di erences in psychosocial development can be theoretically predicted by the parental socialization style. Of the few previous studies that examined the long-term impact of parental socialization, most of them focused on young adults [58], they were conducted in countries with an Anglo-Saxon culture, or they compared adolescents and older adults using di erent adjustment criteria [76]. Therefore, the present study allows a more exhaustive comparison by using five identical key psychosocial criteria and examining the impact of short-term and long-term socialization in three groups of adults. In addition, although it is not the main objective of the study, our results coincide with sex and age di erences found in some previous studies [58,63,109]. Adolescence is frequently characterized as the developmental period in which the physical and social status of a child changes to that of an adult [110] (p. 112). Adolescents are not yet adults, and their parents act as socializing agents by using practices based on warmth and strictness. The present study showed important age-related di erences in the developmental indicators examined in adolescents and adults, but also among young, middle-aged, and older adults, confirming some results from developmental studies about age-related di erences across the life span [78,111]. In adolescence, but especially in adulthood, there are many influences (e.g., biological, personal, social and cultural) on development [57]. Despite these influences, at least in adolescence, previous parenting studies showed that di erences in competence and adjustment among adolescents can be consistently related to parenting styles—in the same cultural context—[11,71]. Much less is known about what happens in adulthood. Nevertheless, as in some previous emergent studies, the main findings revealed that di erences in competence and adjustment among adult children are also related to parenting. As in most of the previous studies with adult children [112,113], the present study follows a cross-sectional design, and so the results should be considered preliminary. Longitudinal evidence is more dicult to obtain for adult children, but the few longitudinal studies [114] have also revealed consistent links between parenting and adult development. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 12 of 17 This study has strengths and limitations. The use of the four-typology parental socialization model provides a common framework with which to compare the results of research conducted in di erent countries around the world. The impact of parental socialization is examined beyond adolescence, with three groups of adult children and five psychosocial development criteria. Therefore, in addition to contributing to the current debate on the best parental socialization strategy, the findings of this study allow us to verify whether di erences in psychosocial development between adults are also theoretically predictable based on parenting styles. As study limitations, it should be noted that the responses were provided by the adolescents and adult children [115], although there is evidence suggesting lower social desirability in children than in their parents [116]. Additionally, participants from the present study only include Spanish families and their adolescent and adult children. Therefore, future studies should extend these findings by examining parenting in immigrant families or ethnic minorities in Spanish society. In addition, the results of this study should be interpreted with caution, without inferring causal relationships or excluding possible third variables (especially considering the time elapsed for older adults), although these results are similar to those found in other studies with adolescents. 5. Conclusions This study makes a valuable contribution to the current international debate about the suitability of parental socialization styles [72,73,117]. On the one hand, the findings of this work coincide with a set of studies, mainly carried out in countries in Europe [53,54] and Latin America [55,56], suggesting that the indulgent style is the optimal parental socialization style. However, the findings of the present study do not coincide with the classic studies mainly carried out with samples of middle-class European-American families [3,6,11,15]. The cultural context where socialization occurs seems to be decisive in the relationship between parental socialization styles and the children’s pattern of competence and adjustment. Future studies should examine the most appropriate parental strategy to socialize children considering families from di erent ethnic and cultural contexts [118–121], as well as di erent individual characteristics of the children [122,123]. Author Contributions: Conceptualization, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; methodology, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G. and; software, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; validation, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; formal analysis, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; investigation, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; resources, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; data curation, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; writing—original draft preparation, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; writing—review and editing, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; visualization, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; supervision, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; project administration, J.A.M.-E., S.V., O.F.G. and F.G.; funding acquisition, O.F.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript. Funding: This research was partially funded by grants ACIF/2016/431 and BEFPI/2017/058, which provided funding for a research stay at the Nottingham Trent University, UK (Valencian Regional Government, and European Social Fund), and FPU16/00988 (Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities, Government of Spain). Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest. References 1. Maccoby, E.E.; Martin, J.A. Socialization in the context of the family: Parent–child interaction. In Handbook of Child Psychology; Mussen, P.H., Ed.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1983; Volume 4, pp. 1–101. 2. Baumrind, D. Reciprocal Rights and Responsibilities in Parent-Child Relations. J. Soc. Issues 1978, 34, 179–196. [CrossRef] 3. Maccoby, E.E. The Role of Parents in the Socialization of Children—An Historical Overview. Dev. Psychol. 1992, 28, 1006–1017. [CrossRef] 4. Baumrind, D. Parental Disciplinary Patterns and Social Competence in Children. Youth Soc. 1978, 9, 239–276. [CrossRef] 5. Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E. Raising Children with Poor School Performance: Parenting Styles and Short- and Long-Term Consequences for Adolescent and Adult Development. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1089. [CrossRef] Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 13 of 17 6. Baumrind, D. Current patterns of Parental Authority. Dev. Psychol. 1971, 4, 1–103. [CrossRef] 7. Baumrind, D. E ects of Authoritative Parental Control on Child Behavior. Child Dev. 1966, 37, 887–907. [CrossRef] 8. Baumrind, D. Authoritarian Vs Authoritative Parental Control. Adolescence 1968, 3, 255–272. 9. Martinez, I.; Garcia, F.; Veiga, F.; Garcia, O.F.; Rodrigues, Y.; Serra, E. Parenting Styles, Internalization of Values and Self-Esteem: A Cross-Cultural Study in Spain, Portugal and Brazil. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2370. [CrossRef] 10. Martínez, I.; Cruise, E.; Garcia, O.F.; Murgui, S. English Validation of the Parental Socialization Scale—ESPA29. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1–10. [CrossRef] 11. Darling, N.; Steinberg, L. Parenting Style as Context: An Integrative Model. Psychol. Bull. 1993, 113, 487–496. [CrossRef] 12. Martínez, I.; Garcia, F.; Musitu, G.; Yubero, S. Family Socialization Practices: Factor Confirmation of the Portuguese Version of a Scale for their Measurement. Rev. Psicodidact. 2012, 17, 159–178. 13. Martinez, I.; Garcia, F.; Fuentes, M.C.; Veiga, F.; Garcia, O.F.; Rodrigues, Y.; Cruise, E.; Serra, E. Researching Parental Socialization Styles Across Three Cultural Contexts: Scale ESPA29 Bi-Dimensional Validity in Spain, Portugal, and Brazil. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 197. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 14. Schaefer, E.S. A Circumplex Model for Maternal Behavior. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1959, 59, 226–235. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 15. Lamborn, S.D.; Mounts, N.S.; Steinberg, L.; Dornbusch, S.M. Patterns of Competence and Adjustment among Adolescents from Authoritative, Authoritarian, Indulgent, and Neglectful Families. Child Dev. 1991, 62, 1049–1065. [CrossRef] 16. Garcia, F.; Gracia, E. The indulgent parenting style and developmental outcomes in South European and Latin American countries. In Parenting across Cultures; Selin, H., Ed.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2014; pp. 419–433. 17. Symonds, P.M. The Psychology of Parent-Child Relationships; Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, NY, USA, 1939. 18. Baldwin, A.L. Behaviour and Development in Childhood; Dryden Press: New York, NY, USA, 1955. 19. Freud, S. New Introductory Lectures on Psycho-Analysis; Hogarth Press: London, UK, 1933. 20. Rogers, C.R. A Therapist’s View of Personal Goals. (Pendle Hill Pamphlet 108); Pendle Hill: Oxford, UK, 1960. 21. Becker, W.C.; Krug, R.S. A Circumplex Model for Social-Behavior in Children. Child Dev. 1964, 35, 371–396. [CrossRef] 22. Sears, R.R.; Maccoby, E.E.; Levin, H. Patterns of Child Rearing; Row, Peterson: Evanston, IL, USA, 1957; p. 549. 23. Watson, J.B. Psychological Care of Infant and Child; G. Allen & Unwin: London, UK, 1928. 24. Steinberg, L. Psychological control: Style or substance? In New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development: Changes in Parental Authority during Adolescence; Smetana, J.G., Ed.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2005; pp. 71–78. 25. Garcia, O.F. Parental Socialization in Spain Across the Life Span. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain, 2019. Available online: https://hdl.handle.net/10550/72456 (accessed on 1 June 2020). 26. Axpe, I.; Rodri guez-Fernandez, A.; Goni, E.; Antonio-Agirre, I. Parental Socialization Styles: The Contribution of Paternal and Maternal a ect/communication and Strictness to Family Socialization Style. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2204. [CrossRef] 27. Alonso-Stuyck, P. Which Parenting Style Encourages Healthy Lifestyles in Teenage Children? Proposal for a Model of Integrative Parenting Styles. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2057. [CrossRef] 28. Gómez-Ortiz, O.; Romera, E.M.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Del Rey, R. Parenting Practices as Risk Or Preventive Factors for Adolescent Involvement in Cyberbullying: Contribution of Children and Parent Gender. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 2664. [CrossRef] 29. Barber, B.K.; Olsen, J.E.; Shagle, S.C. Associations between Parental Psychological and Behavioral-Control and Youth Internalized and Externalized Behaviors. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 1120–1136. [CrossRef] 30. Silk, J.S.; Morris, A.S.; Kanaya, T.; Steinberg, L. Psychological Control and Autonomy Granting: Opposite Ends of a Continuum or Distinct Constructs? J. Res. Adolesc. 2003, 13, 113–128. [CrossRef] 31. Grusec, J.E.; Lytton, H. Social Development: History, Theory, and Research; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1988. 32. Garcia, F.; Fernández-Doménech, L.; Veiga, F.H.; Bono, R.; Serra, E.; Musitu, G. Parenting styles and parenting practices: Analyzing current relationships in the Spanish context. In Parenting: Cultural Influences and Impact Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 14 of 17 on Childhood Health and Well-Being; Garcia, F., Ed.; Nova Science Publishers, Inc.: Hauppauge, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 17–31. 33. Tur-Porcar, A.; Jimenez-Martinez, J.; Mestre-Escriva, V. Substance use in Early and Middle Adolescence. The Role of Academic Ecacy and Parenting. Psychosoc. Interv. 2019, 28, 139–145. [CrossRef] 34. Lewis, C.C. The E ects of Parental Firm Control: A Reinterpretation of Findings. Psychol. Bull. 1981, 90, 547–563. [CrossRef] 35. Baumrind, D. Rejoinder to Lewis Reinterpretation of Parental Firm Control E ects: Are Authoritative Families really Harmonious? Psychol. Bull. 1983, 94, 132–142. [CrossRef] 36. Kerr, M.; Stattin, H. What Parents Know, how they Know it, and several Forms of Adolescent Adjustment: Further Support for a Reinterpretation of Monitoring. Dev. Psychol. 2000, 36, 366–380. [CrossRef] 37. Stattin, H.; Kerr, M. Parental Monitoring: A Reinterpretation. Child Dev. 2000, 71, 1072–1085. [CrossRef] 38. Steinberg, L.; Lamborn, S.D.; Dornbusch, S.M.; Darling, N. Impact of Parenting Practices on Adolescent Achievement: Authoritative Parenting, School Involvement, and Encouragement to Succeed. Child Dev. 1992, 63, 1266–1281. [CrossRef] 39. Barber, B.K. Parental Psychological Control: Revisiting a Neglected Construct. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 3296–3319. [CrossRef] 40. Arrindell, W.A.; Akkerman, A.; Bagés, N.; Feldman, L.; Caballo, V.E.; Oei, T.P.S.; Torres, B.; Canalda, G.; Castro, J.; Montgomery, I.; et al. The Short-EMBU in Australia, Spain, and Venezuela. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2005, 21, 56–66. [CrossRef] 41. Perris, C.; Jacobsson, L.; Lindstrom, H.; Knorring, L.V.; Perris, H. Development of a New Inventory for Assessing Memories of Parental Rearing Behavior. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1980, 61, 265–274. [CrossRef] 42. Arrindell, W.A.; Sanavio, E.; Aguilar, G.; Sica, C.; Hatzichristou, C.; Eisemann, M.; Recinos, L.A.; Gaszner, P.; Peter, M.; Batagliese, G.; et al. The Development of a Short Form of the EMBU: Its Appraisal with Students in Greece, Guatemala, Hungary and Italy. Pers. Individ. Di er. 1999, 27, 613–628. [CrossRef] 43. Tur-Porcar, A.; Mestre, V.; Llorca, A. Parenting: Psychometric Analysis of Two Studies in Spanish Population. Anu. Psicol. 2015, 45, 347–359. 44. Schaefer, E.S. Childrens Reports of Parental Behavior: An Inventory. Child Dev. 1965, 36, 413–424. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 45. Chao, R.K. Beyond Parental Control and Authoritarian Parenting Style: Understanding Chinese Parenting through the Cultural Notion of Training. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 1111–1119. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 46. Chao, R.K. Extending Research on the Consequences of Parenting Style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 1832–1843. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 47. Zayas, L.H.; Solari, F. Early-Childhood Socialization in Hispanic Families: Context, Culture, and Practice Implications. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 1994, 25, 200–206. [CrossRef] 48. Torres-Villa, M.S. Parenting Styles, Language and Parents’ Education as Predictors of School Achievement for Hispanic Students. Ph.D. Thesis, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA, 1995. 49. Baumrind, D. An Exploratory Study of Socialization E ects on Black Children: Some Black-White Comparisons. Child Dev. 1972, 43, 261–267. [CrossRef] 50. Deater-Deckard, K.; Dodge, K.A.; Bates, J.E.; Pettit, G.S. Physical Discipline among African American and European American Mothers: Links to Children’s Externalizing Behaviors. Dev. Psychol. 1996, 32, 1065–1072. [CrossRef] 51. Dwairy, M.; Achoui, M. Introduction to Three Cross-Regional Research Studies on Parenting Styles, Individuation, and Mental Health in Arab Societies. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2006, 37, 221–229. [CrossRef] 52. Dwairy, M.; Achoui, M.; Abouserfe, R.; Farah, A. Parenting Styles, Individuation, and Mental Health of Arab Adolescents: A Third Cross-Regional Research Study. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2006, 37, 262–272. [CrossRef] 53. Garcia, F.; Serra, E.; Garcia, O.F.; Martinez, I.; Cruise, E. A Third Emerging Stage for the Current Digital Society? Optimal Parenting Styles in Spain, the United States, Germany, and Brazil. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2333. [CrossRef] 54. Calafat, A.; Garcia, F.; Juan, M.; Becoña, E.; Fernández-Hermida, J.R. Which Parenting Style is More Protective against Adolescent Substance use? Evidence within the European Context. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014, 138, 185–192. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 55. Martínez, I.; Garcia, J.F.; Yubero, S. Parenting Styles and Adolescents’ Self-Esteem in Brazil. Psychol. Rep. 2007, 100, 731–745. [CrossRef] [PubMed] Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 15 of 17 56. Martínez, I.; Garcia, J.F. Internalization of Values and Self-Esteem among Brazilian Teenagers from Authoritative, Indulgent, Authoritarian, and Neglectful Homes. Adolescence 2008, 43, 13–29. [PubMed] 57. Villarejo, S.; Martinez-Escudero, J.A.; Garcia, O.F. Parenting Styles and their Contribution to Children Personal and Social Adjustment. Ansiedad Estrés 2020, 26, 1–8. [CrossRef] 58. Garcia, O.F.; Lopez-Fernandez, O.; Serra, E. Raising Spanish Children with an Antisocial Tendency: Do we Know what the Optimal Parenting Style is? J. Interpers. Violence 2018. [CrossRef] 59. Martínez, I.; Murgui, S.; Garcia, O.F.; Garcia, F. Parenting in the Digital Era: Protective and Risk Parenting Styles for Traditional Bullying and Cyberbullying Victimization. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 90, 84–92. [CrossRef] 60. Riquelme, M.; Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E. Psychosocial Maladjustment in Adolescence: Parental Socialization, Self-Esteem, and Substance use. An. Psicol. 2018, 34, 536–544. [CrossRef] 61. Alonso-Geta, P.M.P. Parenting Style in Spanish Parents with Children Aged 6 to 14. Psicothema 2012, 24, 371–376. 62. Fuentes, M.C.; Alarcón, A.; Gracia, E.; Garcia, F. School Adjustment among Spanish Adolescents: Influence of Parental Socialization. Cult. Educ. 2015, 27, 1–32. [CrossRef] 63. Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E.; Zacares, J.J.; Garcia, F. Parenting Styles and Short- and Long-Term Socialization Outcomes: A Study among Spanish Adolescents and Older Adults. Psychosoc. Interv. 2018, 27, 153–161. [CrossRef] 64. Musitu-Ferrer, D.; León-Moreno, C.; Callejas-Jerónimo, E.J.; Esteban-Ibáñez, M.; Musitu-Ochoa, G. Relationships between Parental Socialization Styles, Empathy and Connectedness with Nature: Their Implications in Environmentalism. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2461. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 65. Martínez, I.; Fuentes, M.; Garcia, F.; Madrid, I. The Parenting Style as Protective or Risk Factor for Substance use and Other Behavior Problems among Spanish Adolescents. Adicciones 2013, 25, 235–242. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 66. Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E.; Zacares, J.J.; Calafat, A.; Garcia, F. Alcohol use and Abuse and Motivations for Drinking and Non-Drinking among Spanish Adolescents: Do we Know enough when we Know Parenting Style? Psychol. Health 2020, 35, 645–664. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 67. Perez-Gramaje, A.F.; Garcia, O.F.; Reyes, M.; Serra, E.; Garcia, F. Parenting Styles and Aggressive Adolescents: Relationships with Self-Esteem and Personal Maladjustment. Eur. J. Psychol. Appl. Leg. Context 2020, 12, 1–10. [CrossRef] 68. Garaigordobil, M.; Aliri, J. Parental Socialization Styles, Parents’ Educational Level, and Sexist Attitudes in Adolescence. Span. J. Psychol. 2012, 15, 592–603. [CrossRef] 69. Moreno-Ruiz, D.; Martinez-Ferrer, B.; Garcia-Bacete, F. Parenting Styles, Cyberaggression, and Cybervictimization among Adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 93, 252–259. [CrossRef] 70. Chumakov, M.; Chumakova, D. Parents’ Personality, Marriage Satisfaction, Stress, and Punishment of Children in the Family. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 153. [CrossRef] 71. Garcia, F.; Gracia, E. Is always Authoritative the Optimum Parenting Style? Evidence from Spanish Families. Adolescence 2009, 44, 101–131. 72. Pinquart, M.; Gerke, D.C. Associations of Parenting Styles with Self-Esteem in Children and Adolescents: A Meta-Analysis. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2019, 28, 2017–2035. [CrossRef] 73. Pinquart, M.; Kauser, R. Do the Associations of Parenting Styles with Behavior Problems and Academic Achievement Vary by Culture? Results from a Meta-Analysis. Cult. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 2018, 24, 75–100. [CrossRef] 74. Perez-Aranda, G.I.; Peralta-Lopez, V.; Estrada-Carmona, S.; Garcia Reyes, L.; Angel Tuz-Sierra, M. Attachment and Adulthood in a Sample of Southeastern Mexico. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 134. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 75. Flotskaya, N.; Bulanova, S.; Ponomareva, M.; Flotskiy, N.; Konopleva, T. Self-Identity Development among Indigenous Adolescents from the Far North of Russia. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 106. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 76. Sta ord, M.; Kuh, D.L.; Gale, C.R.; Mishra, G.; Richards, M. Parent-Child Relationships and O spring’s Positive Mental Wellbeing from Adolescence to Early Older Age. J. Posit. Psychol. 2016, 11, 326–337. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 77. Barthomew, K.; Horowitz, L. Attachment Styles among Young-Adults—A Test of a 4-Category Model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1991, 61, 226–244. [CrossRef] 78. Baltes, P. Theoretical Propositions of Life-Span Developmental-Psychology—On the Dynamics between Growth and Decline. Dev. Psychol. 1987, 23, 611–626. [CrossRef] Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 16 of 17 79. Reigal, R.E.; Moral-Campillo, L.; Morillo-Baro, J.P.; Juarez-Ruiz de Mier, R.; Hernandez-Mendo, A.; Morales-Sanchez, V. Physical Exercise, Fitness, Cognitive Functioning, and Psychosocial Variables in an Adolescent Sample. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1100. [CrossRef] 80. Guarnido, A.J.S.; Cabrera, F.J.H.; Osuna, M.J.P. Eating Disorder Detection through Personality Traits and Self-Concept. Eat. Weight Disord. Stud. Anorex. Bulim. Obes. 2012, 17, e309–e313. 81. Maiz, E.; Balluerka, N. Trait Anxiety and Self-Concept among Children and Adolescents with Food Neophobia. Food Res. Int. 2018, 105, 1054–1059. [CrossRef] 82. Martín-Albo, J.; Núñez, J.L.; Navarro, J.G.; Grijalvo, F. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Translation and Validation in University Students. Span. J. Psychol. 2007, 10, 458–467. [CrossRef] 83. Cornella-Font, M.; Vinas-Poch, F.; Juarez-Lopez, J.R.; Malo-Cerrato, S. Risk of Addiction: Its Prevalence in Adolescence and its Relationship with Security of Attachment and Self-Concept. Clin. Health 2020, 31, 21–25. [CrossRef] 84. Holubcikova, J.; Kolarcik, P.; Geckova, A.M.; Reijneveld, S.A.; van Dijk, J.P. The Mediating E ect of Daily Nervousness and Irritability on the Relationship between Soft Drink Consumption and Aggressive Behaviour among Adolescents. Int. J. Public Health 2015, 60, 699–706. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 85. Scholte, R.; van Lieshout, C.; van Aken, M. Perceived Relational Support in Adolescence: Dimensions, Configurations, and Adolescent Adjustment. J. Res. Adolesc. 2001, 11, 71–94. [CrossRef] 86. Eisenberg, N.; Strayer, J. Empathy and its Development; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1987; p. 406. 87. Eisenberg, N.; Eggum, N.D.; Di Giunta, L. Empathy-Related Responding: Associations with Prosocial Behavior, Aggression, and Intergroup Relations. Soc. Issue Policy Rev. 2010, 4, 143–180. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 88. Paciello, M.; Fida, R.; Tramontano, C.; Cole, E.; Cerniglia, L. Moral Dilemma in Adolescence: The Role of Values, Prosocial Moral Reasoning and Moral Disengagement in Helping Decision Making. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 10, 190–205. [CrossRef] 89. Sortheix, F.M.; Schwartz, S.H. Values that Underlie and Undermine Well-being: Variability across Countries. Eur. J. Pers. 2017, 31, 187–201. [CrossRef] 90. Balliet, D.; Joireman, J.; Daniels, D.; George-Falvy, J. Empathy and the Schwartz Value System: A Test of an Integrated Hypothesis. Individ. Di er. Res. 2008, 6, 269–279. 91. Pérez, J.F.G.; Navarro, D.F.; Llobell, J.P. Statistical Power of Solomon Design. Psicothema 1999, 11, 431–436. 92. Gracia, E.; Garcia, F.; Musitu, G. Macrosocial Determinants of Social Integration: Social-Class and Area E ect. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 5, 105–119. [CrossRef] 93. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.G. Statistical Power Analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for Correlation and Regression Analysess. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [CrossRef] 94. Rohner, R.P.; Saavedra, J.; Granum, E.O. Development and Validation of the Parental Acceptance Rejection Questionnaire: Test Manual. Cat. Sel. Doc. Psychol. 1978, 8, 17–48. 95. Ali, S.; Khaleque, A.; Rohner, R.P. Pancultural Gender Di erences in the Relation between Perceived Parental Acceptance and Psychological Adjustment of Children and Adult O spring: A Meta-Analytic Review of Worldwide Research. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2015, 46, 1059–1080. [CrossRef] 96. Rohner, R.P.; Khaleque, A. Reliability and Validity of the Parental Control Scale: A Meta-Analysis of Cross-Cultural and Intracultural Studies. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2003, 34, 643–649. [CrossRef] 97. Garcia, F.; Musitu, G. AF5: Self-Concept Form 5; TEA editions: Madrid, Spain, 1999; p. 39. 98. Fuentes, M.C.; Garcia, F.; Gracia, E.; Lila, M. Self-Concept and Drug use in Adolescence. Adicciones 2011, 23, 237–248. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 99. Murgui, S.; García, C.; García, A.; Garcia, F. Self-Concept in Young Dancers and Non-Practitioners: Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the AF5 Scale. Rev. Psicol. Deporte 2012, 21, 263–269. 100. Garcia, J.F.; Musitu, G.; Veiga, F.H. Self-Concept in Adults from Spain and Portugal. Psicothema 2006, 18, 551–556. 101. Garcia, F.; Martínez, I.; Balluerka, N.; Cruise, E.; Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E. Validation of the Five-Factor Self-Concept Questionnaire AF5 in Brazil: Testing Factor Structure and Measurement Invariance Across Language (Brazilian and Spanish), Gender, and Age. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2250. [CrossRef] 102. Garcia, J.F.; Musitu, G.; Riquelme, E.; Riquelme, P. A Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the “Autoconcepto Forma 5” Questionnaire in Young Adults from Spain and Chile. Span. J. Psychol. 2011, 14, 648–658. Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 101 17 of 17 103. Chen, F.; Garcia, O.F.; Fuentes, M.C.; Garcia-Ros, R.; Garcia, F. Self-Concept in China: Validation of the Chinese Version of the Five-Factor Self-Concept (AF5) Questionnaire. Symmetry 2020, 12, 798. [CrossRef] 104. Garcia, F.; Gracia, E.; Zeleznova, A. Validation of the English Version of the Five-Factor Self-Concept Questionnaire. Psicothema 2013, 25, 549–555. 105. Greenberger, E.; Josselson, R.; Knerr, C.; Knerr, B. The Measurement and Structure of Psychosocial Maturity. J. Youth Adolesc. 1975, 4, 127–143. [CrossRef] 106. Schwartz, S.H. Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 25, 1–65. 107. Musitu-Ferrer, D.; Esteban Ibáñez, M.; León, C.; Garcia, O.F. Is School Adjustment Related to Environmental Empathy and Connectedness to Nature? Psychosoc. Interv. 2019, 28, 101–110. [CrossRef] 108. Veiga, F.H.; Garcia, F.; Reeve, J.; Wentzel, K.; Garcia, O.F. When Adolescents with High Self-Concept Lose their Engagement in School. Rev. Psicodidact. 2015, 20, 305–320. [CrossRef] 109. Lila, M.; Gracia, E.; Garcia, F. Ambivalent Sexism, Empathy and Law Enforcement Attitudes towards Partner Violence against Women among Male Police Ocers. Psychol. Crime Law 2013, 19, 907–919. [CrossRef] 110. Baumrind, D. E ective parenting during the early adolescent transition. In Advances in Family Research Series. Family Transitions; Cowan, P.A., Herington, E.M., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1991; pp. 111–163. 111. Babore, A.; Trumello, C.; Candelori, C.; Paciello, M.; Cerniglia, L. Depressive Symptoms, Self-Esteem and Perceived Parent-Child Relationship in Early Adolescence. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 982. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 112. Buri, J.R. Parental Authority Questionnaire. J. Pers. Assess. 1991, 57, 110–119. [CrossRef] 113. Buri, J.R.; Louiselle, P.A.; Misukanis, T.M.; Mueller, R.A. E ects of Parental Authoritarianism and Authoritativeness on Self-Esteem. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1988, 14, 271–282. 114. Flouri, E. Psychological Outcomes in Midadulthood Associated with Mother’s Child-Rearing Attitudes in Early Childhood—Evidence from the 1970 British Birth Cohort. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2004, 13, 35–41. [CrossRef] 115. Besi, M.; Sakellariou, M. Teachers’ Views on the Participation of Parents in the Transition of their Children from Kindergarten to Primary School. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 124. [CrossRef] 116. Barry, C.T.; Frick, P.J.; Grafeman, S.J. Child Versus Parent Reports of Parenting Practices: Implications for the Conceptualization of Child Behavioral and Emotional Problems. Assessment 2008, 15, 294–303. [CrossRef] 117. Alvarez-Garcia, D.; Carlos Nunez, J.; Garcia, T.; Barreiro-Collazo, A. Individual, Family, and Community Predictors of Cyber-Aggression among Adolescents. Eur. J. Psychol. Appl. Leg. Context 2018, 10, 79–88. [CrossRef] 118. Assari, S.; Gibbons, F.X.; Simons, R.L. Perceived Discrimination among Black Youth: An 18-Year Longitudinal Study. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 44. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 119. Assari, S. Race, Intergenerational Social Mobility and Stressful Life Events. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 86. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 120. Queiroz, P.; Garcia, O.F.; Garcia, F.; Zacares, J.J.; Camino, C. Self and Nature: Parental Socialization, Self-Esteem, and Environmental Values in Spanish Adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3732. [CrossRef] [PubMed] 121. Zhang, X.; Gatzke-Kopp, L.; Fosco, G.M.; Bierman, K.L. Parental Support of Self-Regulation among Children at Risk for Externalizing Symptoms: Developmental Trajectories of Physiological Regulation and Behavioral Adjustment. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 528–540. [CrossRef] 122. Maleki, M.; Mardani, A.; Chehrzad, M.M.; Dianatinasab, M.; Vaismoradi, M. Social Skills in Children at Home and in Preschool. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 74. [CrossRef] 123. Aslamazova, L.A.; Muhamedrahimov, R.J.; Vershinina, E.A. Family and Child Characteristics Associated with Foster Care Breakdown. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 160. [CrossRef] © 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

Journal

Behavioral SciencesMultidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute

Published: Jun 16, 2020

There are no references for this article.